Top of Page
The Prohibition Of Touch In PsychoTherapy
Numerous cultural, political, religious and professional forces affect our attitudes toward touch in general and in psychotherapy in particular. These forces co-contribute to the general sense that touch in therapy is an inappropriate, even dangerous behavior that should be avoided or at the least, when unavoidable, held to a minimum.
Cultural-political-religious forces:
- The general western culture and its emphasis on autonomy, independence, separateness and privacy results in restricting interpersonal physical touch to a minimum.
- The cultural tendency in the US to sexualize most forms of touch facilitates confusion differentiating between medical, sensual and erotic or sexual types of touch.
- The lack of differentiation between sensual and sexual touch is more pronounced for men in this culture than for women, as boys are homophobically socialized to avoid gentle and sensual forms of touch. American men are socialized to be more familiar with violent, aggressive, drunken, reckless or sexual forms of touch. Acceptable forms of touch for men also include contact involved in sports and military action.
- Attention to sexual abuse of children in general, heightened by the recent exposure of sexual exploitation of children by clergy, has given teachers, counselors, caregivers, ministers and other authority figures a new level of fear concerning touching.
Professional psychotherapeutic forces:
- The traditional dualistic Western mind-body or mental-physical split manifests itself in Western medicine, including psychotherapy. Typifying this split, are questions that are common in the field of therapy, such as “Is it organic or functional?” or “Is it mental or physical?” The lack of integration of the physical and mental models makes the exploration of touch difficult.
- The traditional psychoanalytic emphasis on the analyst’s neutrality and distance and the focus on clear, rigid, inflexible boundaries omit touch as a therapeutic possibility. (For an excellent historical review of attitudes toward touch in therapy, see Bonitz, 2008)
- Several feminist scholars have asserted that due to patriarchal values and inherent differences in power between men and women, most, if not all touch by male therapists of female clients has a disempowering effect on the woman.
- The fear-based paranoid notion, promoted by the slippery slope idea, that non-sexual touch on the part of the therapist inevitably leads to sexual relationships and exploitation discourages therapists from utilizing touch.
- Risk management, or defensive medicine, focuses on avoiding any therapist conduct that may appear questionable in court or in front of boards or ethics committees, regardless of clinical appropriateness and effectiveness.
Top of Page
Beyond Verbal Communication
Verbal communication is only one of many forms of human communication (Knapp & Hall, 2001; Montagu, 1971; Westland, 2015). Using our senses, humans consciously or unconsciously employ many non-verbal forms of communication, primarily visual, tactile and intuitive. Frank (1970) asserts that language never completely supersedes the more primitive forms of communication, such as voice tone and physical touch. Still, graduate and professional education pays almost no attention to non-verbal communication (O’Connor, 2017). Most psychotherapists are wedded to the spoken word and often rigidly focus on and adhere to verbal communication. Researchers have intensely studied non-verbal communication with children and adults and in courting behaviors. Neuroscientists have demonstrated the interplay of body, mind, and emotions (Berendsen, 2017). Very little of the resulting knowledge, however, has
been incorporated into traditional psychotherapy training or practice. Furthermore, practitioners have voiced concerns over the lack of knowledge and training when using touch in their work with clients, pointing towards the need for training, supervision, and continuing education regarding this facet of therapeutic practice (Burkholder et al., 2010; Courtney & Sui, 2018).
Charles Darwin published the first scientific study of non-verbal communication in 1872 in The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Since that time, thousands of research projects in a variety of fields, such as archaeology, biology, cultural and physical anthropology, linguistics, primatology, psychology, psychiatry, and zoology, have been conducted. The results of this research have established a generally recognized body of knowledge of non-verbal cues and communication. Recent discoveries in neuroscience provide us with an even clearer picture of the importance of non-verbal communication. Because we now know how the brain processes non-verbal cues, body language has come of age in the 21st Century as a science to help us understand what it means to be human (Givens, 2004). Early theorists attended to non-verbal cues, but little was written regarding ways in which these elements could be brought into sharper awareness and focus until the pioneering works of Reich (1972) and Perls (1973). Few modern clinical training programs address this vital dimension in much depth (Blatner, 2002; LaPierre, 2012).
Psychiatrists and psychologists have found that disturbances in non-verbal communication are more severe and often longer lasting than disturbances in verbal language (Bateson, 1979). From paleocircuits in the spinal cord, brain stem, basal ganglia, and limbic system, cues are produced and received below the level of conscious awareness. Non-verbal communication gives us the sense and the feel we remember long after words have died away (Givens, 2004). Many cultural influences involve an unspoken rule that people should ignore non-verbal elements of communication, so the task of incorporating conscious sensitivity and awareness to non-verbal communication is often limited. Generally speaking, women are superior to men in decoding non-verbal cues, most likely for biological evolutionary reasons.
Categories of Non-verbal Communication
Categories of non-verbal communication include internal cues and physiological responses. The elements of these categories are often not practiced voluntarily. Somatic and body psychotherapists assist their clients in becoming more aware of these subtle signs of emotion. Over 80 non-verbal elements of communication arising from the face and head and a further 55 produced by the body have been documented. Clinicians can learn to be aware of these reactions in themselves and educate their clients in “body voice” awareness. This can lead to rich discussion and gained insights regarding the meaning of what these emotional communications could mean. Following are several categories and their elements.
Somatic Expression: These “body voice” communications include: Blushing, perspiring, changes in breathing, flushing, muscle tension (constriction or expansion), moisture in eyes, blanching, flaring of nostrils, unconscious movement of body parts, visceral experience, numbing, and temperature changes (Blatner, 2002).
Personal Space: Personal space is also a form of non-verbal communication. Many factors affect the distance in which individuals experience comfort in approaching or being approached by others. Power structures, role relations, gender, cultural factors, social relationships, location (e.g. public or private), personal and familial factors, and past experience with boundary violations are all included in this category.
Eye Contact: Eye contact modifies the meaning of other non-verbal behaviors. For example, people on elevators or in crowds adjust their sense of personal spatial comfort if they agree to limit their eye contact (Scheflen, 1972).
Paralanguage: “Non-lexical” vocal communications suggest emotional nuances. These include, but are not limited to, inflection, intensity, tone, pitch or pauses.
Facial Expressions: Transient facial expressions may communicate emotions that are not intended or conscious. The face is an extremely developed organ of expression.
Gestures: Gestures are also often unconscious or unintended modes of non-verbal communication. These might include, among many others: raised eyebrows, narrowing eyes, touching one’s face, folding arms, pursing lips, self-hugging, or changes in breathing.
Adornment: Semiontics is the science of the emotional and psychological impact of signs and appearances. These elements might include: clothing, makeup, pens, pipes, belts, pillows, etc. (Blatner, 2002).
Therapeutic goals can be served when therapists increase their awareness of these forms of non-verbal communication and educate their clients about them as well. This self-knowledge is empowering to clients.
People vary in regard to the sense mode they primarily rely on, whether it is auditory, visual, olfactory or tactile. It is essential to pay careful attention to the type of communication that is most effective with each client. A therapist who is sensitive to this issue might sit with a family and ask the father, “Did you hear what was just said?” Then ask the mother, “Did you see what just happened”? Then ask the son, “Did you notice what just took place?” And finally ask the daughter, “Did you sense what just happened?” Similarly, people will react to the same stimuli by using different terms, such as “bad taste,” “ugly looking,” “stinks” or “sounds horrible.” The intent to honor individual differences is compromised in an exclusive focus on verbal communication.
Top of Page
Touch In Context
The meaning of touch can only be understood within the context of the client, the therapeutic relationship, and the therapeutic setting (Warnecke, 2011; Zur, 2007a, 2007b). Accordingly, the employment of touch must be carefully considered in its context. Touch can have radically different contextual meanings amongst different clients, therapists, and settings (Hilton, 1997; Horton, et al., 1995, Koocher & Keith-Spiegel, 1998; McGrane, 2019; Smith et al., 1998). What is highly appropriate and effective with one client in a certain situation may be inappropriate
and even damaging with another. Hugging a child or letting him or her jump onto the therapist’s lap may be highly appropriate in the course of child therapy, but not in the case of a borderline client. Reaching out gently and respectfully to hold the hand of a grieving mother may not have the intended positive effect if the same is done in early stages of therapy with a female survivor of sexual abuse.
Following is an example of the importance of context. Imagine a 50-something male therapist who tells you that he spent a recent therapy session holding hands with a recumbent 24-year-old female client he had only just met. Of course in a “standard” therapeutic situation such a scene is likely to evoke associations of an unethical, unprofessional, most likely illegal and inappropriate sexual mode. Now imagine that this exchange takes place in a hospital room. The patient has cystic fibrosis, severe lung disease, and struggles mightily to breathe and speak despite the oxygen pressure mask she wears. The therapist asks how he can help, and she grips his hand tightly saying, “Don’t let go.” (Koocher, personal communication, 2004).
Gender issues are extremely important in understanding the context of touch. As noted above, men in general are more likely to sexualize touch unless it is hostile or aggressive (Smith et al., 1998). Along the same line, Abbey and Melby (1986) found that men are more likely to perceive sexual intent when women touch them, while women are less likely to perceive such sexual intent in men, especially when the situation is ambiguous or casual.
In large part, clients’ individual factors constitute the context of touch and therefore are of extreme importance. They include presenting problem, diagnosis, personality, history, and in particular, history of abuse, culture and gender. Timing is as important (Warnecke, 2011). While a handshake may be appropriate at the beginning of treatment, other forms of touch, such as a hug or a kiss on the cheek, may not be. It is essential for the therapist to have explored his or her own relationship to touch issues as well.
Even Pope and Vasquez (1998), with their major concerns about boundaries and dual relationships, have articulated the potential benefits of touch in psychotherapy:
If the therapist is personally comfortable engaging in physical contact with a patient, maintains a theoretical orientation for which therapist-client contact is not antithetical, and has competence (education, training and supervised experience) in the use of touch, then the decision of whether or not to make physical contact with a particular client must be based on a careful evaluation of the clinical needs of the client at that moment. When solidly based upon clinical needs and a clinical rationale, touch can be exceptionally caring, comforting, reassuring, or healing (p. 170).
Top of Page
Clients’ Perceptions Of Touch
Clients, sometimes independent of therapists’ intentions, construct the meaning of touch. For example, a light touch on the arm by a therapist meaning to be supportive and affirming may be experienced by a client either, as intended, supportive, warm, encouraging and affirming, or it may be perceived as hostile, intrusive, controlling and disrespectful. Similarly, a handshake at the end of the session can be perceived as a gesture of respect and affirmation or as indication of the therapist’s coldness, rigidity, distance or dislike of the client. A therapist’s hug, meant to be supportive, may be experienced as affirming and calming, or as overwhelming, intrusive or as sexual harassment.
Non-erotic or non-sexual touch in therapy, like any other form of non-verbal communication, can complement, negate, reinforce or alter verbal communication in general and in therapy (Geib, 1982; Horton et al., 1998; Phalan, 2009; Smith et al, 1998; Young, 2005; Zur, 2007a, 2007b). Many of our clients, like people in general, are often more attuned to non-verbal cues such as body language and touch rather than to verbal communication (Givens, 2004). The quality of attunement between client and therapist is of utmost importance regarding the meaning of non-verbal cues and will determine the clients’ response to touch.
Geib (1982) conducted one of the first phenomenological studies of the meaning attributed by clients with regard to non-sexual touch and isolated four factors that are associated with a client’s positive evaluation of touch in therapy: Congruence of touch; clarity regarding boundaries in therapy; client’s perception of being in control of the physical contact; and, client’s perception that the touch was for his/her benefit rather than the therapist’s. A study done by Horton and colleagues (1995) supported Geib’s finding that the degree of therapeutic alliance significantly influences the client’s evaluation of touch. Citing multiple studies, Guindon and colleagues (2017) illustrated clients’ positive perceptions of touch in therapy with the following: touch can be reassuring to clients, leading to increased engagement in the process; helps the client view the therapist as more trustworthy; helps elicit ease in disclosure; and, helps establish, preserve, and deepen the therapeutic alliance. Hoffman’s (2016) dissertation concerning positive experiences of touch in therapy among survivors of sexual trauma demonstrated the necessity of trust and safety with the therapist, which are permitted provided certain client-therapist relationship qualities, including the client’s sense of empowerment surrounding touch and perception of genuine care as well as the therapist’s capacity to accurately witness and attune to the client.
Studies have been conducted on therapists’ perspectives with regards to touch in therapy. Harrison and colleagues (2012) conducted a phenomenological analysis that resulted in five main themes from interviews with psychologists who described offering or withholding touch in sessions with adult clients: the touch instinct; touch and professional boundaries; individual clients and contexts; the value of touch in therapy; and, the cost of touch in therapy. Similarly, O’Keefe’s (2016) phenomenological analysis of therapists’ perceptions of touch in therapy demonstrated that therapists’ perceived effectiveness resulted from not only having already established a therapeutic alliance before proceeding with touch, but also found that perceived effectiveness was enhanced when therapists’ actualized their own embodied awareness as well as balance risks and potential benefits when proceeding.
Top of Page
Touch And The Therapeutic Alliance
Several researchers report how touch enhances therapeutic alliance. In a study by Horton and colleagues (1995), clients used terms and phrases, such as “bond,” “safety,” “closeness,” “there for me,” “on my side,” “deepened trust” and “my therapist really cares about me” to describe feelings associated with being touched by the therapist. Some considered being touched an indication of the therapist’s emotional availability and others described experiencing “parity” with the therapist. The same survey also found that many clients stated that touch reinforced their sense of the therapist’s caring about them, which in turn allowed them to open up and take more risks in therapy, hence increasing the benefits of treatment.
Therapists’ congruent, invited, and appropriate non-sexual touch is likely to increase a sense of empathy, sympathy, safety, calm, and comfort for clients (McGuirk, 2012; Smith et al., 1998). It can also significantly enhance clients’ sense of being heard, seen, understood and acknowledged by their therapists. As such, appropriate touch is likely to increase the sense of connection and trust between a therapist and a client as well as a sense of self and sense of belonging (Berendsen, 2017). Research indicates that the clients’ experience of touch in psychotherapy provides valuable information on assessing the therapeutic alliance and predicting therapy outcome (Gurman, 1977; Hartly & Strupp, 1983; Lambert, 1991; Marziali, 1984; Salvio, et al., 1992). Hilton (1997) describes the importance of touch for transference and countertransference analysis.
Guindon and colleagues (2017) cited a study offering therapist qualities that hinder the use of touch in therapy, including as follows:
Psychotherapists who (1) experience a crisis in their lives (e.g., marital conflict); (2) live life transitions (e.g., retirement, job change); (3) are ill; (4) experience loneliness; (5) have feelings of arrogance (e.g., feel they are above the law), of shame (e.g., be reticent to consult in case of doubt) or envy towards their clients (e.g., a counter-transference related to beauty, youth, intelligence, and the status of the client); (6) have difficulty putting limits in place within the therapeutic relationship; and (7) refuse to admit to problems within the therapeutic relationship (e.g., are in denial) (p. 219).
Top of Page
Surveys on Touch In Therapy
In an interesting twist of logic, while professional literature, ethics classes and risk management principles advocate avoiding touch as much as possible, many surveys report that most therapists touch their clients in a non-sexual manner. Tirnauer, Smith and Foster (1996) report that 87% of therapists touch their clients. A total of 85% reported by Pope, Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel (1987) hug their clients rarely or sometimes. Schultz (1975) found that 65% of therapists approve of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy. Holroyd and Brodsky (1977) found that approximately one third of psychologists reported using some form of touch with their clients.
Pope, Tabachnick, Keith-Spiegel (1987) report the following from their survey of psychotherapists:
About one fourth of our respondents reported kissing their clients, either rarely (23.5%) or more often (5.0%). About half viewed this practice as unethical. An additional 36.6% believed it to be unethical in most circumstances.
Hugging clients was practiced by 44.5% of the respondents on a rare basis, and by an additional 41.7% more frequently. Few (4.6%) believed the practice to be clearly unethical, but 41.2% believed it to be ethical only under rare circumstances.
The findings in the previous two categories may be compared to the results reported by Holroyd and Brodsky (1977) in which 27% of the therapists reported occasionally engaging in nonerotic hugging, kissing, or affectionate touching with opposite-sex patients, and 7% reported doing so frequently or always.
Almost all respondents offered or accepted a handshake from a client, either rarely (48.9%) or more frequently (48.2%). Very few found the behavior to be ethically questionable (p.1001).
The study of attitudes towards touch in therapy primarily involves issues related to sexual feelings, sexual touch and sexual boundary violations. Most of these surveys present several methodological concerns either because they seem to lack clear differentiations between sexual and non-sexual touch or assume that, in most cases, non-sexual touch inevitably leads to sexual touch. The bias against boundary crossing in general and touch in particular, combined with sampling limitations have also been a concern with the validity of the surveys. Holroyd and Brodsky (1980) titled their article regarding their survey, “Does touching patients lead to sexual intercourse?” Not surprisingly they also concluded that it “is difficult to determine where ‘non-erotic hugging, kissing, and affectionate touching’ leave off and ‘erotic contact’ begins” (p. 810). Similarly, Pope and various colleagues (Pope & Bouhoutsos, 1986; Pope, et al., 1980; Pope & Vasquez, 1998) have conducted several surveys that gave much more weight to the potential dangers of touching and how it is likely to lead to sexual intimacy rather than the more inquisitive balanced stance of looking at the positive and negative potentialities and unique complexities that touch introduces into therapy. The bias of Pope, Tabachnick and Keith-Spiegel (1987) in regard to touch is apparent as soon as one notices that under the heading “Do Not Exploit,” they discuss the topics of “Sexual issues and physical contact” under one subtitle. In their conclusion, citing several of their colleagues, they state, “The focus on erotic contact in therapy has raised questions about the legitimacy and effects of ostensibly nonerotic physical contact” (p. 1001).
In their national survey, Pope and Tabachnick (1994) were admittedly surprised to find that nonsexual physical touch in the form of being cradled or held by their therapists was reported by about one fourth of client-participants. They also found that about 1 in 20 (6%) reported that the therapist touched the client in a sexual way. These researchers’ bias in regard to the issue of touch in therapy becomes apparent when one reads their following reflection and their alarm at their finding:
That so many participants had been cradled or held by their therapists was surprising, in part, because of the attention paid, by both the public and the profession, to sexual contact. In light of the harm and penalties associated with sexual violations, it might be hypothesized that therapists would tend to avoid forms of physical contact such as holding and cradling (i.e., those beyond a handshake, reassuring touch, and other brief and limited forms of making contact). The finding is also somewhat surprising given the relevant lack of therapy research literature on the effects of nonphysical touch. That at least one fourth of the participants reported having been cradled or held by their therapists suggests that this form of therapist-client relationship deserves increased attention in systematic studies, graduate training programs, and continuing education courses (Pope & Tabachnick, 1994, p. 257)
The prevalent biased view in the field has, in part, been perpetuated by these writers who do not consider touch as an important and basic healing method, or recognize it as one of the most basic forms of human communication. Like so many others in the field, they associate or link non-sexual touch with sexual touch. Additionally, their statement that there is a ‘lack’ of research of the healing effect of touch is as surprising as it is inaccurate. This attitude goes along with Pope’s (1982) and his followers’ assertion that non-sexual dual relationships often lead to sexual dual relationships.
Perhaps one of the more important significant findings is reported by Holroyd and Brodsky (1980). Their study relates to the differential treatment of female and male clients with regard to non-erotic touch, which was systematically related to therapist-client erotic involvement. The important conclusion of these finding is that therapists’ own attitudes towards touch and their tendency to sexualize touch are the key factors in contributing to sexual misconduct rather than the touch itself. This has been interpreted by most researchers to mean that therapists who differentiate between those with whom they will employ touch in therapy are sexualizing touch and are also more likely to violate the sexual boundaries of therapy. The same study did not
find any correlations between non-sexual touch and sexual touch when therapists touched men and women equally. The important conclusion of these findings is that therapists’ own attitudes towards touch and their tendency to sexualize touch are the key factors in contributing to sexual misconduct rather than the touch itself. A more balanced view of touch and its potential benefits and risks is found in surveys of therapists and clients, presented in a series of chapters found in Smith et al.’s (1998) book.
As was cited above, several factors have been found to significantly correlate with a client’s positive evaluation of touch, such as clarity regarding boundaries, congruence of touch, client’s sense of being in control and the client’s perception that touch is for his/her benefit (Geib, 1982; Horton et al., 1995; McGuirk, 2012). Positive functions of touch, as expressed by clients who have experienced touch in psychotherapy, include providing a link to external reality, increasing self-esteem, and inviting the experience of new modes of relating. Other surveys of clients who have experienced touch in psychotherapy indicate that touch reinforced their sense of the therapist’s caring and involvement. The findings also “support the judicious use of touch with clients who manifest a need to be touched, or who ask for comforting or supportive contact” (Horton et al., 1995, p.455).
Top of Page
Clinical Orientations on Touch
Body psychotherapists’ clinical orientation, such as Reichian (Reich, 1972) or Bioenergetics (Lowen, 1958, 1976) use touch as their primary tool in psychotherapy. They see the value of touch and endorse it as a therapeutic tool whole-heartedly.
Most psychoanalysts, as noted earlier, are highly opposed to any form of touch in therapy (Menninger, 1958; Smith, et al. 1998; Wolberg, 1967). However, many other orientations support the clinically appropriate use of touch (Milakovitch, 1993; Williams, 1997; Young, 2005; Zur, 2007a, 2007b). Very few modern analysts, such as Fosshage (2000), have differed with the main line analytic doctrine and advocate a clinically responsible use of touch in psychoanalysis.
The human potential movement and the humanistic movement of the 1960s introduced a whole new approach to touch and boundaries in therapy. This movement endorsed appropriate non-erotic touch and viewed it as an enhancement of the therapist-client connection (Bonitz, 2008; Hunter & Struve, 1998). Rogers (1970) discusses the value of touch and describes specifically how he has soothed clients by holding, embracing and kissing them. Gestalt therapy incorporates numerous forms of touch as an integral part of therapy (Perls 1973). Gestalt practitioners place a special importance on non-verbal communication and non-verbal intervention. Unfortunately, Gestalt practices in the 60’s and early 70’s, under Perls’ leadership went too far and at times, included unethical sexual touch in conjunction with therapy (Hunter & Struve, 1998). Lazarus and Zur (2002) note how the promiscuous practices in the 1960s around touch, nudity and sexuality have resulted in some form of backlash, where touch and other boundary crossings have since been viewed as harmful.
Family therapists, including Satir (1972) often utilize touch as an element of engaging clients in therapy (Holub & Lee, 1990). Behavioral and cognitive behavioral therapists are likely to incorporate touch or any boundary crossing into therapy if it fits with their interventions, such as modeling or reinforcement (Lazarus & Zur, 2002).
Feminist therapists seem to have two camps when it comes to issues of boundaries, including touch. The more vocal, politically and professionally active faction focuses on issues of power, male dominance, sexuality, and oppressive patriarchal values. Predictably, they take a strong stance against most boundary crossings and dual relationships and advocate for the protection of, what they see as, vulnerable female clients sexually exploited by powerful male therapists. Borys, Bouhoutsos, Brown, Keith-Spiegel, Jorgenson, Kitchener, Sonne, and Vasquez are some of the authors who tend to align with this ideology (Zur, 2005). Alyn (1988) describes a kind of touch hierarchy in which it is more likely for a person of higher status to touch someone of lower status than the converse. In this context any touch, even non-sexual touch by male therapists of women clients is seen as disempowering and therefore harmful to women. The much less vocal faction of feminist therapy focuses on essential issues of inclusion, connection, mutuality, self-disclosure, and equality. The focus of these writers, as manifested in the important work of Greenspan (1995) and some contributions by The Stone Institute and The Feminist Therapy Institute (1987), relate how healing often entails tearing down rigid, arbitrary, professional boundaries rather than erecting them. Along these lines, appropriate touch, which is congruent with the therapist-client relationship, is seen as potentially healing.
In their survey of therapists, Pope, Tabachnick and Keith-Spiegel (1987) report that therapists of differing theoretical orientations have very different beliefs about the effect and practice of touching clients. They report that 30% of humanistic therapists indicated that nonerotic hugging, kissing and affectionate touching might frequently benefit clients in psychotherapy. In contrast, only 6% of psychodynamic therapists indicated the same. While most psychodynamic therapists thought touch could be easily misunderstood, humanistic therapists did not share this view.
Milakovitch (1998) compares therapists who touch and those who do not touch. The following are some of his findings:
- Therapists who touch are likely to subscribe to a humanistic theoretical orientation, while therapists who do not touch usually subscribe to a psychodynamic orientation.
- Therapists who touch, obviously value touch in therapy and believe that gratifying the need to be touched is important. Therapists who do not touch believe that gratifying the need to be touch is detrimental to therapy and the client.
- Unlike therapists who do not touch, therapists who touch were more likely to be touched by their own therapists and had supervisors and professors who believe in the legitimacy of touch as a therapeutic tool.
- Therapists who touch were more likely to experience body psychotherapies than therapists who do not touch.
- Unlike therapists who do not touch, therapists who touch are (obviously) more supportive of models that use touch and employ body psychotherapy techniques.
- Female therapists tend to touch their clients more often than do male therapists.
Top of Page
Sexual And Non-Sexual Touch In Therapy
Most writers have defined touch as the physical contact between a therapist and a client, either directly on the person’s skin or through the person’s clothing (Pope, et al., 1993). However, many writers have struggled with mapping the boundaries between sexual and non-sexual touch in therapy. Some focus on the areas touched (i.e., hand vs. genitals). Others focus on whether the intent is to sexually arouse the client, oneself, or not. Yet others attempt to differentiate between overt vs. covert touching behavior. Some, like Brodsky (1985), propose an encompassing broad view that “erotic touch” should be defined as any behavior that leads to sexual arousal.
Part of the problem with differentiating sexual and non-sexual touch in therapy stems from the lack of differentiation between sexual feeling and sexual activity. While about 90% of therapists report being sexually attracted to their clients at some time (Pope & Vasquez, 1998), less than 10% have ever violated their clients sexually. Lazarus and Zur (2002) and Smith et al.,
(1998), like many other writers, emphasize that the problem of such lack of differentiation is rooted in insufficient professional education. Part of the problem with differentiating sexual and non-sexual touch in therapy stems from the lack of differentiation between sexual feeling and sexual activity. They view the problem as starting with graduate schools, which focus on rigid, restrictive ethical education and the teaching of risk management practices rather than providing a focus which will assist students in recognizing and processing their sexual feeling towards clients, something, which most would agree, is a common element in the therapist/client dynamic (Pope et al., 1993). Such lack of education undoubtedly exacerbates the problem, resulting in untrained therapists who tend to deny difficult or unacceptable feelings in a process, which is likely to increase their vulnerability to violate their clients.
Top of Page
Body-centered Therapies: History, Reichian, Bioenergetics, Radix, Somatic Experiencing
Historically, there has been a centuries-long profound split between body and mind in Western thought and in psychotherapy in general. It has just been in the last twenty-five years that the correspondence between physiological and psychological processes has found form in somatically-based psychotherapies (Young & Westland, 2014). Body-centered psychotherapies view the body/mind as a feedback loop or continuum rather than two separate systems, recognizing that any event experienced impacts us in a holistic way—physically, cognitively, spiritually, and emotionally. Healthy functioning and dysfunction in any part of the organismic continuum will affect the whole system (Caldwell, 1997).
The United States Association for Body Psychotherapy (USABP) (2003) asserts, “All experiences, as well as distortions and denials of reality and other defensive maneuvers, are reflected not only in peoples’ thoughts and feelings but also in the way they move, how they breathe and how the structure of their bodies has evolved over the years”(Para. 1). Body psychotherapy assists people in healing and developing not only through the use of verbal interventions, but also through guiding them to a deeper awareness of their bodily sensations, images, behavior and feelings. There are many approaches to body psychotherapy just as there are multiple approaches in psychotherapy and a variety of techniques are employed. “These forms seek to resensitize our clients to their birthright of healthy and optimal functioning by using the direct physical experience of the body as a healing tool. These systems also advocate our continued growth and transformation as humans through reclaiming our integrative being” (Caldwell, 1997, p. 26). Techniques common to most body-centered psychotherapies include attention to somatic awareness, breath, movement, imagery, and touch, which can vary from deep manipulation used to release body blocks to supportive hugs or holding.
Humanistic, Existential and Gestalt psychology, as well as dance and movement therapy, family therapy, systems theory, biology, and Far Eastern philosophy have all contributed to body psychotherapy approaches. All attempt to integrate the body and mind and take into account when problems began and how they effect a person’s development over the years.
Dr. Don Johnson, (Johnson & Grand, 1998), the director of the Somatic Graduate program at the California Institute of Integral Studies, (San Francisco, CA), summarizes well his attempt to integrate the field of somatic therapy:
Somatics is a generative concept like “cognitive science,” “ecology,” or QiGong, names whose function is to create possibilities of collaboration among communities that otherwise stand isolated from each other, often in competition and conflict. For example, Reichian bioenergetics, Rolfing, and hatha yoga once seemed worlds apart. Now with the help of this new paradigm, we can now see how each can augment and make more effective the practice of the others. The elaboration of this idea has created a worldwide community whose members, remaining typically grounded in their own unique contexts of skill and service, profit from the contributions of the others. The word itself derives from the ancient Greek soma, once used to describe the whole person (Johnson, 2010, Somatic, retrieved from http://www.donhalonjohnson.com/somatics.html).
The History of Body Psychotherapy
Surprisingly, it was Freud who laid the groundwork for body psychotherapy, as he identified the body and body processes as the foundation of psychological states (Caldwell, 1997). He described the ego as being first and foremost a body ego, and he taught that the physical blocking or discharge of energy is essential in the formation of psychological disorders. He subsequently became fascinated with verbal analysis and the only aspect of his somatic perspective that remained a part of his focus was his technique of working with his clients as they lay on a couch. He felt this position relaxed tense musculature and regressed clients to earlier states of development by lowering their defenses.
Ferenczi, originally trained in the psychoanalytic model, at one point spoke out as a proponent of hugging, holding, kissing and non-erotic fondling of clients, believing that the use of such therapeutic touch would provide corrective parenting to clients with early injuries. Initially, Freud was supportive of his experiential experiments but withdrew his support when he became aware that Ferenczi had become sexually and romantically involved with more than one of his clients. Ferenczi refused to discontinue his use of touch and was subsequently expelled from the ranks of orthodox psychoanalysis (Fosshage, 2000).
Reichian Therapy
Wilhelm Reich, a student of Freud, is often referred to as the grandfather of body-oriented psychotherapy, however, a long history of body-oriented approaches to healing, predate his work. One of Reich’s most significant contributions has been his effort to dismantle the barriers and restrictions to touch that had been imposed by the domineering influences of psychoanalysis (Hunter, Struve, 1998). Reichs’ view that modern society functions as a repressive force that results in the basis for all illness (Reich, 1986) contrasted with Freuds’ concept of libido, a form of unsocialized energy, which must be controlled (Freud, 1960). Reich also added the dimension of the body to Freud’s model of ego and internal conflict, in that he saw the ego as controlling impulses and emotions through physiological patterns, e.g. a holding jaw, a tight belly etc. (Eiden, 2002). His development of character analysis correlated psychological and physical patterns. In this context, “character” is seen as a defense against strong emotions and has the function to bind anxiety in the form of muscular tension, e.g. the “fight or flight” response which is a specific reaction to stress, an instinctive reflex which, if unexpressed, stays in the body in the form of a postural holding pattern. Such holding patterns or “blocking” served to protect the individual against painful and threatening emotional experiences (Reich, 1972). Reich’s basic technique was to reduce body armor by palpating or pressing certain muscle groups to dissolve muscle tension to free inhibited energy. This represented a radical departure from the rigid tenants of traditional psychoanalysis by initiating direct physical contact with his clients, for which he was censured and eventually excommunicated from the psychoanalytic community (Older, 1982).
Bioenergetics
Lowen (1958) studied with Reich, but focused on the larger realm of pure feelings instead of emphasizing concerns with orgasmic performance as Reich had. Reichians work with the hypothesis that there is one fundamental energy in the human body, whether it manifests itself in psychic phenomena or in somatic motion. This energy is simply called “bioenergy” (Hunter & Struve, 1998). Both Reich and Lowen believed that organistic potency was a criterion for cure, but Lowen included the ability to express all emotion fully. He worked to increase the client’s awareness of unconscious fears and conflicts in relationship to tensions and rigidities in the body. To this end, he developed exercises to help the client magnify and release tensions in the body, thus freeing blocked emotions. He used pressure on muscles, expressive exercises, and breath work and worked with dreams, memories and emotions that might emerge from the unconscious as a result of the bodywork (Lowen, 1976). Stanley Keleman (1987) broke from mainstream bioenergetics in his articulation of how movement creates the body and the body creates movement. He works with breath, movement and sound as he examines the vibratory processes of the body down to the cellular level. He believes that the quality of this pulsation shapes our physical form. He seeks to reestablish charge, formation, and discharge in a process that develops healthy tissue and holistically healthy individuals.
Radix
Charles Kelley, the founder of the Radix institute, describes “Radix” as the source from which energy, feeling, and movement are created and his work is less analytical and verbally oriented than Reichian therapy or Bioenergetics. There is more focus on how a person blocks fear, anger or painful emotions rather than on content. He uses visual techniques to open the ability to access deep, spontaneous emotion and to choose appropriate goals, increasing self-direction, control and significance in the life of his students. For Kelley, the focus is on education and growth. Most Radix work is done in groups (Caldwell, 1997).
Somatic Experiencing (SE)
Somatic Experiencing (SE) is a naturalistic approach to the resolution and healing of trauma developed by Dr. Peter Levine, Founder and Senior Advisor for The Foundation for Human Enrichment in Niwot, CO (Levine & Frederick, 1997). The SE modality is based on the observation that wild prey animals, though threatened routinely, are rarely traumatized. Animals in the wild, according to SE theory, utilize innate mechanisms to regulate and discharge the high levels of energy arousal associated with defensive survival behaviors. These mechanisms provide animals with a built-in, natural or innate immunity to trauma that enables them to return to normal in the aftermath of highly “charged” life-threatening experiences.
According to the Foundation for Human Enrichment, Somatic Experiencing is not considered as a form of psychotherapy, it stands on its own as an approach to healing trauma. The Foundation is clear that SE is neither a psychotherapy nor a bodywork technique, but lends itself well to being integrated into these and other treatment modalities.
Focusing
The term focusing was popularized by Dr. Eugene Gendlin in the 1960’s. Focusing refers to the simple matter of holding an open, non-judging attention to something which is directly experienced but is not yet in words. Focusing pays attention to the methods of inner awareness called “felt sense,” a quality of engaged, accepting attention, and a philosophy of what facilitates change. The Focusing technique can be successfully used in any kind of therapeutic situation, including peer-to-peer sessions (Gendlin, 1982).
Dr. Stanley Keleman, Formative Psychology
Formative psychology was developed by Dr. Keleman (1981, 1987). He describes as follows:
Formative psychology, is based in the evolutionary process in which life continually forms the next series of shapes, from birth through maturity to old age. At conception each person is given a biological and emotional inheritance, but it is through voluntary effort that a human fulfills the potential for forming a personal life. Form gives rise to feeling. When individual identity is grounded in somatic reality, we can say: I know who I am by how I experience myself.
Formative psychology gives a philosophy and method of how to work with our life. We learn to regenerate our emotional and instinctual vitality, to inhabit our body, and to incorporate our excitement and emotional aliveness. The goal of formative practice is to use daily life to practice being present and to create an adult self and reality. I proceed from the premise that we are each conceived as an adult and that we grow the adults we are meant to be.
Additional approaches to body psychotherapy
Other pioneers in the field have blended disciplines to form their work. This includes, but are not limited to, such disciplines as: The Lomi School, which mixes Gestalt therapy with Reichian breath work, body education, biodynamic psychotherapy, Meditation, Yoga and Tai Chi and Stanley Groff’s Holotropic breath work.
New forms of body-centered psychotherapy are evolving, which apply softer techniques and less analytical methodology. These forms use less exploitive, stressful postures, invasive touching, or breathing to extreme states. There is less of a focus on analysis as the client takes more responsibility for finding meaning in the communication from their body voice. These new forms, yet to be thoroughly researched, include Kurtz’s (1990) Hakomi Therapy, Gay and Kathy Hendricks’ Radiance Method, Amy and Arnold Mindell’s (1983) Process Therapy, Christine Caldwell’s (1997) Moving Cycle, the Rubenfeld Synergy Method (Rubenfeld, 2000), the Alexander Technique, Marion Woodman’s BodySoul Rhythms, Aline LaPierre’s NeuroAffective Touch, and Pat Ogden’s Sensorimotor Psychotherapy, among others.
Biochemical Links between Consciousnesses, Mind & Body Energy
Somatic therapists refer to “energy” and associate it with the release of emotion and the restoration of health. This is a foreign concept to most Western, traditionally-trained practitioners, but ancient and alternative healing methods refer to a force of energy that animates the entire organism. Chiropractors refer to it as “innate intelligence”, Hindus call it Prana, Chinese, chi, Freud, libido, Reich, orgone energy. Candace Pert (1997), a neuroscientist, states, “It’s my belief that this mysterious energy is actually the free flow of information carried by the biochemicals of emotion, the neuropeptides and their receptors” (p. 288). The limbic system, often referred to as the part of the brain that controls emotions, has forty times more neuropeptide receptors than other parts of the brain. Blood flow is closely regulated by emotional peptides, which signal receptors on blood vessel walls to constrict or dilate, and so influence the amount and velocity of blood flowing through them from moment to moment. The brain requires a plentiful source of glucose in order for the neurons and glial cells to perform their function. When emotions are blocked due to denial, repression, or trauma, blood flow can become chronically constricted, depriving the frontal cortex, as well as other organs, of vital nourishment. This can cause one to feel foggy and less alert, limited in awareness, with diminished ability to facilitate the body-mind conversation in order to make conscious decisions that alter physiology or behavior. Hence, one becomes stuck repeating old patterns of emotion and behavior. “Work that is both somatic and emotional fosters self-healing by giving clients access to the limbic system” (Caldwell, 1997, p. 193). The nervous system learns from pleasure as well as pain. Each time we make sense of new information, the brain rewards us by releasing endorphins and other pleasure-producing petrochemicals. We are familiar with these concepts as they relate to behavioral classical conditioning, and we are familiar with the common “aha” experience in psychotherapy. Touch is a very sophisticated language that is communicated through our skin, both receiving and giving information. It bypasses words and rational concepts housed in the neocortical brain (Caldwell, 1997).
Pert (1997) postulates:
The limbic system, and it’s neurons have axons that extend into the pituitary gland, these axons secrete a neuropeptide called CRF. . . We could say that CRF is the peptide of negative expectations, since it may have been stimulated by negative experiences in childhood. . . Autopsies almost always show a tenfold higher level of CRF in the cerebrospinal fluid of those who killed themselves compared to those who died from other causes (p. 270).
Animal studies show that monkey babies deprived of maternal nurturing or are neglected or abused have high levels of CRF. These baby animals were cured by an older ‘monkey hug therapist’ who simply cuddled and hugged the stressed out baby monkeys causing their chronically elevated CRF levels to go down.
In the case of treating mood disorders and other mental unwellness, the mainstream misses a lot by excluding touch, by ignoring the fact that the body really is a gateway to the mind, and by refusing to acknowledge the importance of emotional release as a mind-body event with the potential to supplement or even sometimes replace talk cures and prescription pills (Pert, 1997, p. 274). Furthermore, as Dr. Lubin-Alpert of the Hartford Family Institute suggests, “Not to touch any clients at any time can be experienced as abusive as the original neglect to the … infant inside the adult client” (as cited in Norwood, 2017, The Use of Touch in Therapy Today section, para. 3).
Feeling is healed through somatic experience because our minds and our feelings reside in our bodies.
Pert (1997) writes of accessing ones’ psychosomatic network and entering the bodymind’s conversation and redirecting it as a way to keep information flowing, feedback systems working, and natural balance maintained. She asserts that we are literally able to consciously and intentionally intervene at the level of our molecules, making significant changes in our physiology, releasing certain biochemicals into our systems. The well-known tools of traditional psychotherapeutic trade, such as dreams and the symbolic meaning of words, as well as touch do, in fact, access the psychosomatic network.
There is no doubt that these traditional tools have their place and their effectiveness but it is necessary to acknowledge other effective points of entry as well: the skin, spinal cord, and organs are all nodal points of entry into the system. The deepest, oldest messages are stored and must be accessed through the body. “Your body is your unconscious mind, and you can’t heal it by talk alone” (Pert, 1997, p. 306).
Top of Page
Touch With Special Population
Multiple factors effect the decision making process in forming a treatment plan that includes touch . It is crucial to address the specific touch experiences of special populations.
Survivors of Childhood Trauma
The use of touch with survivors of childhood trauma has been much debated. However, the clinically appropriate and ethical use of touch with survivors of childhood abuse can be invaluable in helping them heal and recover from their traumatic experiences (Hunter & Struve, 1998). Due to the nature of their original injuries, many of these clients are likely to feel intense vulnerability at the suggestion of touch in the intimate setting of psychotherapy. There is the possibility that touch used with clients who are survivors of childhood trauma may recreate, or evoke, previous client-experienced dynamics of submission and victimization, entrapment, anger, fear, vulnerability and feelings of worthlessness. Because of this, Lawry (1998) states that the use of touch is contra-indicated in early sessions due to the potential for retraumatization, and Cornell (1997) suggests that language based interventions may allow more adequate time for developing rapport, trust and a sense of safety in which deeper affect and profound pain can be released and tolerated. Once a strong therapeutic alliance has been formed, Cornell (1997) indicates:
The use of touch will evoke, address and hopefully help correct such historical experiences and distortions as: deprivation and neglect; over stimulation, intrusion and bodily violation, sexualization, parental narcissistic use of the child; deadening of vitality or use of the body as an instrument. (p.33).
Studies done in the context of post-traumatic stress disorder have explored the relationship between trauma and memory (Allen, 1995; Herman, 1992; Van der Kolk, et al., 1996). The concept of memory and trauma is highly controversial. A balanced view is provided in a monograph published by The International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies (1998). Many studies document that traumatic memories are encoded in our sensorimotor system as kinesthetic sensations and images. Herman (1992) reports that “in states of high sympathetic arousal, the linguistic encoding of memory is inactivated, and the central nervous system reverts to the sensory and iconic forms of memory that predominate in early life” (p. 39). This results in the client having great difficulty reconstructing a narrative of their traumatic memories as they experience them instead as emotional and sensory recall (Van der Kolk et al., 1996). Due to the somatosensory experience of trauma, new forms of treatment are offered by body-oriented therapies (Hovdestad & Kristiansen, 1996). Bar-Levav (1988) reports that physiological patterns in the body can be changed by touch to correct early injurious experiences.
Since the nature of the original injury, physical and/or sexual assault, involved boundary violation, the therapist must be particularly careful to structure a foundation of client safety and empowerment. Most authors agree that a client must want to touch or be touched and understand the concepts of empowerment before it is clinically or ethically appropriate to begin the use of touch in session (Hunter & Struve, 1998). To this end, clients should be encouraged to express their preferences, to practice boundary-setting exercises, and to participate in creating a treatment plan. Studies suggest that there is a positive correlation between a client’s perception that they are in control of touch and their positive evaluation of touch in psychotherapy (Geib, 1998; Horton, et al., 1995). Many abuse survivors respond to anxiety by having difficulty in protecting their boundaries. Therefore, it is helpful for the therapist to state clearly that he or she accepts full responsibility for ensuring that there will be no sexual contact with the client and to be clear about the process and type of touch that will be involved. Hunter and Struve (1998) suggest that it is helpful to draft written ground rules as many survivors are highly dissociative and have difficulty retaining verbal information in stressful situations.
Helfer (1978) developed a program, which includes a series of graduated exercises through which a client can address developmental tasks that were missed in childhood as a result of abusive or neglectful parenting. Since most survivors of abuse have learned to mistrust touch, he begins by helping the client get in touch with his or her five senses that may have been undeveloped, underdeveloped, or overdeveloped. These exercises explore the distinction between good touch and bad touch and help the client to enjoy the benefits of soothing supportive touch. The therapist may then guide the client in nurturing, supportive self-touch such as self-massage or self-stroking. Another approach is to direct the client in the use of imagery to reconnect with his body, to recognize bodily sensations and to name them. It can also be less threatening for a client to experience safe, nurturing, non-erotic touch through the use of imagery. Clients might further be instructed to deliberately engage in various types of touching activities, such as touching trusted friends or animals, massage, or contact sports.
Any touching in therapy should be solely for the benefit of the client and great caution must be taken if the client is dissociated. The hippocampus, amygdala, hypothalamus and thalamus function by laying down memory traces that are subsequently regulated by stress hormones. Flashbacks can occur when a current stressor activates traumatic memory traces and the client dissociates and loses full contact with essential details in the current environment. Positive therapeutic results have been demonstrated in the cognitive-behavioral treatment of psychological trauma survivors. Touch used in a non-structured, organic manner can yield results similar to those of cognitive-behavioral interventions (Bremner, 2002), with the added benefit that most survivors of abuse report that touch in therapy helps them to feel “touchable” and that appropriate touch in psychotherapy helps them identify appropriate, connective, non- violating forms of touch. “The client can relearn to assimilate sensory data about physical contact only if they are processed concretely and with an intense focus on reality-testing feedback loops” (Hunter & Struve, 1998, p. 217). It is essential that the therapist be familiar with the dynamics of dissociation before working with a trauma survivor. (See Steele & Colrain, 1990, for an extensive list of dissociative signs.) When working with clients who have a history of abuse, it is of crucial importance that the client’s permission be requested prior to making any physical contact. Asking communicates respect for her and her body; it says that her preference will be respected and that no intrusion (however slight) will occur against her will (Courtois, 1998).
Many therapists consider touch of any kind to be inappropriate with clients who have been abused through violations of the body. At the same time however, many therapists and all somatic therapists believe that a client will have great difficulty in fully recovering from such trauma if only verbal or cognitive approaches to therapy are used. As Hunter and Struve (1998) surmise, “Therapeutic efforts to help a client heal from touch-related wounds ultimately ought to include experiential approaches that directly access the body and that provide the client with real-life opportunities to feel nonabusive touch” (p. 218). Clients traumatized in childhood are often unable to make distinctions between affectionate touch and sexual touch (Ball, 2002). “Healing is unlikely to occur if this positive and appropriate touch remains only an idea or intellectual concept” (Hunter & Struve, 1998, p. 218). In a study done by Horton and colleagues (1995), it was found that sexually abused clients were more likely to attribute a corrective or educative role to touch in therapy and to report feeling “touchable”, “lovable” or generally better about themselves as a result of touch than were non-abused clients. Of the clients in this study who reported a history of sexual and/or physical abuse and neglect, 71% wrote that touch repaired self-esteem, trust, and a sense of their own power or agency, especially in setting limits and asking for what they need.
Durana (1998) states that touch is usually contraindicated for clients who are paranoid, actively hostile or aggressive, or who implicitly or explicitly demand touch. Hunter and Struve (1998) suggest that special care should be taken using touch with populations that have experienced assault, neglect, attachment difficulties, sexual addictions, eating disorders, and intimacy issues. Rothschild (2000) suggests that a better strategy for these clients is to assist them in learning how to perceive and respect his or her own boundaries and to teach how to meet the needs of touch among close friends and family due to the complication of the possible provocation of transference and countertransference.
Berendsen (2017) offers several core strategies when incorporating the body into work with clients who have a history of abuse, using the acronym SAFETY: S, stabilize and go slowly; A, attunement, awareness, and acceptance; F, focus on the felt sense of the feeling; E, empathize, educate, explore; T, take time, titrate; Y, your needs matter too (p. 89).
Children and Adolescences
Clinicians who do use touch in session, tend to do so in ways that reflect biases inherent in the larger culture. In this regard, it has been found that therapists tend to touch child clients more often than they do their adult clients and that female therapists touch child clients more often than male therapists do (Cowen, et al., 1983). Hyperactive children tend to have negative reactions to being touched (Bauer, 1977), and clinicians are alerted to be aware of the profound social implications of this sensitivity (Thayer, 1982).
Adolescents may be particularly sensitive to dimensions of control with regard to touch and may react negatively to touch that could be interpreted as patronizing or unduly familiar (Jones, 1994; Smith, et al., 1980).
A growing body of literature has linked aggressive, violent, and antisocial behaviors to early childhood touch deprivation (Katsurada, 2012; Mitchell, 1979; Older, 1982). These children grow up with a diminished ability to feel, and a reduced ability to initiate or receive touch (Hunter & Struve, 1998). In one study, the staff of an adolescent treatment program modeled nonsexual, nonviolent touch to incorporate physical contact as an acceptable aspect of the milieu. They found that the adolescents demonstrated a marked decrease in violent and sexual behaviors (Dunne, et al., 1982).
Touch with Elders
The values of Americas’ youth oriented culture result in many elders experiencing touch deprivation. This is in contrast to high touch cultures in which elders are generally cared for at home in the company of extended family. Most people experience some level of decrements in physical faculties and general perceptual skills but the sense of touch generally remains intact for most older people and actually is valued as increasingly important as a source of contact and communication (Hollinger, 1986). The soothing, affirming experience of touch is most important at the beginning and end of ones’ life and generous, nurturing touch can gently facilitate the process of aging and dying with dignity (Hunter & Struve, 1998).
Gender issues
Touch in psychotherapy occurs between female and male clients as well as same-sex therapist-client dyads, but the highest frequency of physical contact occurs between male therapists and their female clients (Brodsky, 1985). In this context, attention should be paid to power dynamics whereby women touched by male therapists might feel devalued because of social stereotypes (Alyn, 1998). The Report of the Task Force on Sex Bias and Sex-role Stereotyping in Psychotherapeutic Practice appropriately cautions that any treatment modality reserved for only one sex may be interpreted as being sexist (Redleaf, 1998). Rigid application of touch along gender lines fits the definition of sexism and is clinically inappropriate.
Research shows that women respond more positively to touch than do men (Whitcher & Fisher, 1979). From birth, American women receive more affectionate touch from males and females and are given greater permission to touch either gender and be touched by either gender. They are more likely to have and expect a broader repertoire of touch. American males are given less affectionate touch in infancy and early childhood and this has been linked with higher rates of violence and aggression in later life (Redleaf, 1998). For most men in our society, touch has been limited to violent and sexual encounters with the exception of rough yet affectionate touch that is allowed in sports and the military (Montagu, 1971). “Some men, therefore, may lack a repertoire of non-sexual, non-violent touch…and may perceive every touch as either an attack or a come-on” (Redleaf, 1998, p. 44). In studies of psychiatric populations, women favor being held over holding, and men, although desiring to be held, do not express this longing with equal intensity to the expression of women (Hollender & Mercer, 1976). Clinicians are cautioned that for men, who generally do not give or receive nonsexual touch, regression transference may be elicited by the use of nonerotic touch in psychotherapy (Downey, 2001).
Top of Page
Traditional Psychoanalytic Prohibition Of Touch In Therapy
Psychoanalysis traditionally has placed an almost total interdiction on physical touch between client and analyst within the analytic arena. Yet touch, based on our largest sensory organ, the skin, provides a fundamental and elaborate form of communication. So important is tactile stimulation for development and maintenance of physiological and psychological regulations that recent research demonstrates that physiological and psychological regulations of persons of all ages are “righted” through physical holding and touch (Fosshage, 2000).
Psychoanalysis, from the days of its inception, has been highly concerned with the effect of physical touch in analysis. The issue erupted, beginning with Freud, back in 1931 scolding Ferenczi for letting a female client kiss him. Freud wrote to Ferenczi:
You have not made a secret of the fact that you kiss your patients and let them kiss you… Now I am assuredly not one of those who from prudishness or from consideration of bourgeois convention would condemn little erotic gratifications of this kind… But that does not alter the fact… that with us a kiss signifies a certain erotic intimacy… Now picture what will be the result of publishing your technique… A number of independent thinkers will say to themselves: Why stop at a kiss? Certainly one gets further when one adopts “pawing” as well, which after all doesn’t make a baby. And then bolder ones will come along who will go further, to peeping and showing and soon we shall have accepted in the technique of analysis the whole repertoire of demiviergerie and petting parties, resulting in an enormous interest in psychoanalysis among both analysts and patients. (Jones, 1957, pp. 163-164, cited in Marmor, 1972, cited in Koocher & Keith-Spiegel, 1998, p. 207).
Clearly Freud felt that physical contact would almost certainly lead to sexual enactments. By his own admission, he was equally concerned with the reputation of psychoanalysis and forced the issue of touch to go underground.
As psychoanalysis emerged, an entire analytic ideology was created around the prohibition of touch. It is based on the conviction that any touch is likely to gratify sexual and instinctual infantile longings or drives, subsequently contaminating the analytic container and nullifying the possibilities for analysis to help the clients work through their issues (Fosshage, 2000).
The effects of touch, like any boundary crossing, such as self-disclosure, gift giving or home visits, is a major concern in therapy, for almost all psychoanalysts and psychoanalytically oriented therapists. Many of them conceive these effects to be intrinsically negative and hence believe that they invariably interfere with and undermine clinical work (Epstein & Simon, 1990; Guindon et al., 2017; Langs, 1976; Simon, 1992). Psychoanalytic theory emphasizes the importance of boundaries and the neutral stance of the analyst. According to traditional analysts, effective
management of transference and other therapeutic work requires clear and consistent boundaries and therefore no physical contact so the analyst can preserve the analytic frame of therapy (Langs, 1988). Transgressions that detract from therapists’ neutrality, such as touch, are said to contaminate the transference and hence are a detriment to analysis. Langs (1976), an avid supporter of rigid and inflexible boundaries, testifies that “poor boundary management” impedes transference work and has other serious ramifications, such as the dilution of the therapist’s influence. Simon (1995) operates from a similar perspective and has numerous publications that epitomize the case against boundary crossings. Adhering to traditional analytic principles, his main guidelines state: “Maintain therapist neutrality…foster psychological separateness of the patient. . . interact only verbally with clients. . . minimize physical contact” (Simon, 1994, p. 514).
The theoretical rationale for barring physical touch in the analytic process is based on the assumption that, in keeping with the pleasure principle, physical touch gratifies the client’s infantile sexual longings and, thereby, fixates the client at an infantile level. Refusal to touch and refusing to provide gratification, forces infantile sexual wishes into awareness that ultimately facilitates their renunciation. To achieve this goal, the analyst must stay neutral and provide a blank screen onto which the client’s childhood fantasies are displaced and projected. Within the classical conception of transference, the goal is to prevent or remove any possible contribution from the analyst to the client’s experience in order to illuminate the client’s intrapsychically generated projections and displacements. Touch is viewed as “muddying the transferrencial waters,” and is likely to nullify analytic effectiveness. Touch is prohibited for it is viewed as an intrusion of the analyst and an interference with the free associational process and therefore the transference analysis (Fosshage, 2000).
While Freud’s rule of abstinence and interdiction on touch has, thus, predominated in the psychoanalytic literature, there have been notable exceptions where physical touch is seen as not only appropriate, but as necessary when dealing with periods of deep regression (Balint, 1968); Winnicott, 1958, 1975), with psychotic anxieties and delusional transference, and with deeply disturbed clients (see Mintz, 1971 who describes the work of Fromm-Reichman and Searles). More recently, additional reports of the facilitative use of touch have emerged in the literature (Fosshage, 2000). Hilton (1997) has also added the importance of touch in psychodynamic psychotherapy and its inclusion in the transference and countertransference analysis.
Top of Page
On Power And Touch In Therapy
The concern with therapist’s power has been a major focus around the issue of touch due to the related concern with exploitation and sexuality (Zur, 2009). The primary rationale for the argument to abstain from touching is that therapists may misuse their power to exploit clients for their own benefit and to the clients’ detriment (Bersoff, 1999; Borys, 1992; Herlihy & Corey, 1992; Pope & Vasquez, 1998; Woody, 1998). The argument is that the power differential enables and, some argue, encourages therapists to sexually exploit their clients.
Kitchener (1988) describes the power differential between therapists and clients as one of the three most important factors in determining the risk of harm to clients engaged in exploitative relationships with their therapists. Similarly, Gottlieb (1993) lists power differentials as the first dimension in the decision-making model for avoiding exploitative relationships in therapy. Pope (Pope & Vasquez, 1998), like his many followers, maintains that because of the power differential, the client is vulnerable and largely incapable of free choice.
The argument of power differential put forth by feminists like Alyn (1988) or Brown (1985) does not view women as capable of asserting or having power in therapeutic relationships. Instead, it views them as weak and defenseless in the hands of their powerful, dominant male therapists.
The concern with therapists’ power is important, as the power differential is true for many (but not necessarily always) therapist-client relationships. Therapists are generally hired for their expertise and this, in most cases, gives them at least some measure of being an expert, with knowledge and information that increases the power advantage over their clients. Additionally, many of our clients seek our help in times of crisis, confusion and increased vulnerability (Hunter & Struve, 1998; Lazarus & Zur, 2002; Smith et al., 1998).
The term ‘power differential’ between therapists and clients has almost become interchangeable with exploitation and harm in the psychotherapeutic ethics literature (Lazarus & Zur, 2002). What is often forgotten in such discussions is that many relationships with a significant differential of power, such as parent-child, teacher-student or coach-athlete, are not inherently exploitative (Zur, 2000, 2007a, 2009). Power is, in itself, neither positive nor negative; it is neutral. Parental power facilitates children’s growth, teachers’ authority enables students to learn, and the influence of coaches, helps athletes to achieve their full athletic potential. Few, if any, marriage, business, friendship, or therapy relationships are truly equal.
Therapists’ power, like that of parents, teachers, coaches, ministers, politicians, policemen, attorneys or physicians, can be used or abused. The Hippocratic Oath, which states, ‘first do no harm’ attends exactly to such dangers. The problem of abusive or exploitative power in therapy is not going to be resolved by avoiding all touch and other boundary crossing in therapy. Tomm (1993) adds “It is not the power itself that corrupts, it is the disposition to corruption (or lack of personal responsibility) that is amplified by the power” (p. 11).
The problem with the argument of power differential is that all patients are portrayed as malleable, weak, and defenseless in the hands of their powerful, dominant, compelling therapists. The disparity in power is regarded as extreme, which is disempowering to the client. It is possible that many therapists cling to the false ideals of the segregated therapy session and avoid dual relationships because it increases their professional status (Dineen, 1996; Zur, 2000, 2001, 2007a). These therapists are thereby imbuing themselves with undue power that can all too easily be translated into exploitation (Zur, 2001). Many therapists work with clients who are much more powerful than they.
Some clients are CEO’s of large corporations, judges, powerhouse attorneys, master mediators or successful entrepreneurs. Often, these clients do not regard their therapists as particularly powerful or persuasive, and their therapists experience them as more powerful and successful than they. Such cases are a prime example of when therapists have to work hard at cultivating an aura of power so as to appear credible.
In summary, therapists must be very careful not to abuse the trust and power they often have in the therapeutic relationships. At the same time it is important that therapists humbly accept that some clients are more powerful than they are and acknowledge the limitation of how much power and influence they really have. We must all remember that power by itself does not corrupt, but lack of personal integrity does. Instead of banning touch so therapists will not misuse their power, we must increase the therapist’s “Integrity Quotient”.
Top of Page
Touch As A Boundary Issue
The intersection of boundaries, touch and psychotherapy presents a unique and complex matter as it involved two types of boundaries (Zur, 2007a). The first one is the distinct boundary of the physical body and the second one is the more illusive concern with psychotherapeutic boundaries. The boundary of the body is clear and well defined by the skin. It is, at once, the demarcation of physical, separate identity as well as the reciprocal experience of connection. While the skin is physically, distinctively defined, the numerous physiological and emotional regulatory systems affected when the skin is touched, are extremely complex and mysterious.
Boundaries in therapy are, at least as complex as the body boundary. Similar to the body boundary, they seem simple to define at first, but a deeper investigation reveals an extremely intricate and mysterious web of connections between therapeutic boundaries and interventions and clients’ emotional, physical and spiritual wellbeing. This section will focus on the boundary issue aspect of touch in psychotherapy.
Boundaries in psychotherapy have been a topic of growing debate in the past several decades, with touch being a central element of the issue. Therapists who touch their clients have often been viewed as problematic and their actions judged as a boundary problem that is often linked to, or equated with, sexuality and harm. Lazarus and Zur (2002) have emphasized the lack of definition of what constitutes appropriate boundaries in psychotherapy. They illuminate that there is a lack of differentiation between boundary crossing and boundary violation. As a result, confusion, false accusations and fear run rampant.
In the field of psychotherapy, there is neither agreement nor a single definition of what constitutes clinically and ethically appropriate boundaries between therapists and clients. Factors, such as culture and personal preferences, can permit such arbitrariness in boundaries (Bellehumeur & Chambers, 2017). Boundary crossing has been confused and equated with boundary violations (Guindon et al., 2017; McGuirk, 2012). Traditionally, boundary crossings in psychotherapy have been defined as any deviation from traditional, strict, hands-off, ‘only in the office,’ emotionally distant forms of therapy (Lazarus & Zur, 2002; Zur, 2007a). Besides physical touch, boundaries in therapy generally refer to issues of therapists’ self-disclosure, length and place of sessions, activities outside the office, gift exchange, bartering, social and other non-therapeutic contact, and various forms of dual relationships.
As Lazarus and Zur (2002) articulated in their book, Dual Relationships and Psychotherapy, boundary violations in therapy are distinctly different from boundary crossings. While boundary violations by therapists are harmful to their clients, boundary crossings are not and can prove to be extremely helpful. Harmful boundary violations occur typically when therapists and clients are engaged in exploitative relationships, such as therapists’ sexual touch with current clients. In contrast to boundary violations, boundary crossings can be an integral part of well-formulated treatment plans. Examples include when a Reichian or Bioenergetics therapist uses hands-on techniques. A handshake, an appropriate pat on the back, handholding or a non-sexual hug are all also legitimate and often helpful boundary crossings. These forms of touch are similar to other common boundary crossings, such as when a therapist makes a home visit to a bed-ridden or immobile elderly client or when a behavioral therapist, as part of systematic desensitization, flies on an airplane with a client who suffers from a fear of flying (Zur, 2002, 2007a). Boundary crossing may be simply seen as a departure from the traditional, rigid psychoanalytic approach or inflexible risk management proceedings.
At the heart of the opposition to touch in therapy is the argument that places immense importance on separation and clear and inflexible boundaries in therapy. Most of the support for this argument comes from ethicists, attorneys, licensing boards, psychoanalysts, and rigid proponents of clinically restrictive risk management practices. These professionals generally view any deviation from these rigid boundaries as a threat to the therapeutic process. They view touch and most other boundary crossings as transgressions and as potential precursors to harm, exploitation and sexual relationships between therapists and clients (Borys, 1994; Brown, 1994; Katherine, 1993; Pope & Vasquez, 1998; Sonne, 1989; Strasburger, et al., 1992).
As noted throughout this paper, the concern with boundaries has been intricately integrated as a primary focus of psychoanalytic theory and practice. For example, Menninger (1961), Epstein (Epstein & Simon, 1990), Langs (1976) and Simon (1992), view any boundary crossing as detrimental to the therapeutic process and to clients themselves. They have advocated an adherence to rigid therapeutic boundaries and oppose most boundary crossings. In fact, many analysts have viewed even appropriate and helpful boundary crossings, such as a comforting hug or hand holding, as poor boundary management.
The concern with boundaries is not limited to analytically oriented therapists. Most ethics texts advocate quite rigid adherence to strict boundaries and view most boundary crossings as detrimental to therapy (Borys & Pope, 1989; Brown, 1985; Kagle & Geibelhausen, 1994; Katherine, 1993; Kitchener, 1988; Pope & Vasquez, 1998; Sonne, 1989).
What is often ignored by almost all analysts, ethicists and risk management experts is the basic fact that therapeutic orientations, such as humanistic, behavioral, cognitive behavioral, family systems, feminist or group therapy, are inclined to endorse boundary crossings, such as physical touch as part of effective treatment (Williams, 1997; Zur, 2007a). Even though cognitive behavioral, family systems and group therapy are currently the most practiced orientations, they are ignored and marginalized when it comes to ethical discussions of boundaries.
The discussion of boundaries almost completely ignores the fact that non-Western cultures often have a different attitude towards boundaries in general and touch in particular (Lazarus & Zur, 2002).
They, therefore, judge the appropriateness of touch differently. Cultures, such Latino, African American or Native American, are more likely to integrate touch into the communication between therapists and clients.
Another unfounded belief about boundaries in general, and most specifically about touch, is the belief in the ‘slippery slope’ idea. As articulated above, this belief claims that minor boundary crossings inevitably lead to boundary violations and sexual relationships (Black, 2017; Lazarus, 1994). A rigid attitude towards boundary crossings in general and particularly towards touch in therapy stems in part from, what has been called “sexualizing boundaries.” This is a distorted cultural and professional view that sees all boundary crossings as sexual in nature (Zur, 2007a).
Touch and many other boundary crossings with certain clients, such as those with borderline personality disorders or other severe disorders, must be approached with caution. Effective therapy with borderline clients, for example, often requires a clearly structured and well-defined therapeutic environment. Histrionic personality disorder and dependent personality disorder are other diagnoses necessitating cautious approach (Guindon et al., 2017). Boundary crossings should be implemented according to the client’s unique needs and the specific situation. It is recommended that the rationale for boundary crossings be clearly articulated and, when appropriate, included in the treatment plan.
Rigid boundaries and rigid avoidance of all forms of touch can conflict with acting in a manner that is clinically helpful to clients. Inflexible boundaries, distance and coldness are incompatible with healing. Lambert (1992) and many others affirm, through outcome research, the importance of rapport and warmth for effective therapy. Boundary crossings, including touch, are likely to increase trust and connection and hence increase the likelihood of success for the clinical work. Whitfield (1993) also describes how the most serviceable boundaries are those that are flexible, as opposed to those that are implemented in such a rigid manner as to cause harm through excessive and inappropriate distance.
In summary, the exclusive reliance on analytic theory and adherence to risk management practices, which results in the eschewal of virtually all forms of touch and boundary crossings, has been detrimental to the overall impact of psychotherapy. Behavioral, cognitive-behavioral, humanistic, group, family and existential therapeutic orientations are the most practiced orientations today. These treatment approaches tend to endorse touch and other boundary crossings, which are considered clear boundary violations by most ethicists, psychoanalysts and risk management advocates (Williams, 1997). In fact, feminist, humanistic and existential orientations view the tearing down of artificial boundaries and introducing touch and other normal human interaction as essential for therapeutic effectiveness and healing (Greenspan, 1995). Clinical interventions, including touch should be constructed and implemented according to the client’s idiosyncratic situation, condition, problems, personality, culture and history. The unduly restrictive analytic risk-management emphasis on clear, rigid and inflexible boundaries and the mandate to avoid touch interferes with human relatedness, and sound clinical judgment, which ought to be flexible and personally tailored to clients’ needs rather than to therapists’ dogma or fear.
Top of Page
Risk management, Prohibition Of Touch & Slippery slope Argument
Risk management has become one of the most influential forces in medicine in general including psychotherapy. Risk management is the process whereby therapists avoid certain behaviors and clinical interventions, not because they are clinically ill advised, unethical, harmful or wrong, but because they may appear improper in front of judges, juries, licensing boards or ethics committees (Gutheil & Gabbard, 1993; Williams, 1997). While the conscientious and ethical therapist carefully weighs the possible risk of any therapeutic intervention, including touch, against its potential benefits, risk-management frightens us into avoiding all “risky behavior” regardless of the likely positive results (Bonitz, 2008; Guindon et al., 2017; Lazarus, 1994; Williams, 1997; Zur, 2007a).
Touch has often been placed at the top of the ‘Do not do’ list. “From the viewpoint of current risk-management principles,” Gutheil & Gabbard (1993) stated, “a handshake is about the limit of social physical contact at this time” (p. 195). Similarly, WebMD (1992) announces:
A Hug-Free Zone: The threat of lawsuits, the already strong language in the APA code, and the general litigiousness of society have prompted many therapists to erect barriers between themselves and their clients when it comes to any physical contact. No more hugs for a sobbing client. No encouraging pats on the back. (Section 2, para. 1).
Like male preschool teachers who no longer hug young children, or camp counselors who would no longer hold a child in their lap for fear of being accused of inappropriate sexual behavior, many therapists, for similar reasons based on fear, have generally abandoned the practice of touching their clients. Defensive medicine, fueled by fear, is the defining forces behind risk management practices. The teaching of risk management principles seems to dominate ethics classes in graduate school and legal-ethical continuing education workshops. They are the foundation of endless attorneys’ columns in our professional organization newsletters and a more recent breed of risk management presentations at our professional conferences. In all these formats, we are told never to hug, pat or hold our clients. Basically, we are told not to touch beyond a handshake and when possible even to avoid a handshake too. Beware! We are told, the slightest deviation from these ersatz commandments will set us on the ‘slippery slope’ to perdition. But even if one has not slipped uncontrollably down the slippery slope, the sheer idea that an action may appear suspicious to licensing boards, ethics committee members, or to administrative or civil courts’ judges is adequate motivation to avoid touching clients, children or adults all the same.
When we listen closely to the risk management dogma, it is clear that no one really disputes the scientific fact, and the common knowledge, that touch is one of the most elementary human ways to relate and can be a powerful method for healing. Nevertheless, we have been frightened out of employing touch and most other forms of boundary crossings, such as self-disclosure, home visits, accepting gifts, bartering and many other behaviors frowned upon by the so-called “risk management” experts. These experts’ advice often goes specifically against the practices of humanistic, cognitive-behavioral, family, feminist and group therapists. Ironically, these are also the orientations most practiced by psychotherapists.
Misleadingly, many of these attorneys, ethicists and so called risk management experts have mislead the therapeutic community, clients and licensing boards and courts to believe that non-sexual touch is unethical and below the standard of care. Unlike most commonly held beliefs, boundary crossings, such as touch are neither unethical nor below the standard of care. Ethics codes of all major psychotherapy professional associations (e.g., AAMFT, 2015; ACA, 2014; APA, 2017; NASW, 2017) neither prohibit boundary crossings in general nor do they prohibit the use of appropriate touch in therapy. For example, the NASW (2017) states that “[s]ocial workers who engage in appropriate physical contact with clients are responsible for setting clear, appropriate, and culturally sensitive boundaries that govern such physical contact” (Section 1.10 Physical contact). All psychotherapy professional codes of ethics view sexual or violent touch with a current client as unethical. For specific details refer to the ‘ethics codes’ stance on touch in therapy (Zur, 2015).
The obvious question then becomes, “Why are behaviors and interventions, such as touch, that are known to be clinically helpful, as well as very natural elements of human interaction, banned from our practices or, at best, driven underground?” The answer lies primarily in the concept, practice and teaching of defensive medicine or risk management.
In principle, nothing is wrong with managing risk if it is done thoughtfully by applying sensible clinical judgment and employing critical thinking rather than paranoid thinking.
There must also be a sound knowledge of the professional codes of ethics and laws of states. All actions and clinical interventions involve some risk. For that matter, we often forget that inaction can be risky and even damaging to clients, as well (McGuirk, 2012). For example, I have been working with a woman who, 10 years prior to our first session, lost her infant son in an automobile accident. In an emergency appointment with a psychiatrist right after the death of her son, as she sobbed uncontrollably, she begged him to hold her. He refused, citing something about professional boundaries. Instead, he prescribed Valium. Eight years later, addicted to Valium and alcohol, she began therapy with me. After an intense few months of therapy, we visited her son’s grave. It was the first time she had visited the grave. There we stood, holding each other and both weeping as she finally started facing her baby’s death and grieving for him and for her years lost in drugged denial. While the psychiatrist followed risk management guidelines to perfection, he also may have inflicted immense harm. Did he sacrifice his humanity and the core of his professional being, to heartless protocol?
All therapists may, of course, with due consideration, attempt to reduce their own risks and the risks to their clients when employing touch in therapy. This is especially important when working with cases involving borderline or dissociative proclivities. This attempt to reduce risk goes side by side with clinical integrity, relevant training, and sound employment of treatment plans. Appropriate risk-benefit analysis requires that therapists ask themselves basic questions, such as “What is to be gained by employing touch and what is there to lose? What do I risk if I do not touch and what do I risk if I do?” It is very important that such an analysis be made for each client and each clinical situation taking into account the specific client’s mental state, presenting problem, history, culture, personality, communal support, class, gender, etc. (Complete guidelines for the use of touch in therapy will be found at the end of this article.)
Malpractice insurance carriers represent the primary force behind risk management, or what some attorneys call “healthy defensiveness”. While the actual likelihood of a lawsuit or of licensing discipline for psychotherapists is extremely low (Williams, 1997), in the rare event that it takes place, it can be very costly to the insurance company and emotionally and financially devastating to the practitioner. The rare, but nevertheless outrageously costly, judgments drive the malpractice insurance companies to advocate strict risk management practices and the avoidance of any behavior that may give a jury reason to suspect inappropriate behavior and levy an expensive penalty. Ironically, this strategy, as will be discussed later, is more likely to backfire on the insurance companies.
Aiding and abetting the insurance companies and attorneys has also fueled the risk management fire, inspiring paranoia and widespread instruction in risk avoidance behavior. Attorneys’ advice columns seem to have become a regular feature in our professional journals. Scheduled sessions with legal professionals abound at our professional conferences. Often, without any clinical training whatsoever, they sternly give us long lists of what we should avoid. At the top of the list is, of course, “Do not touch!” Obviously the list does not stop there. They tell us never to leave the office even though going to an open space with an agoraphobic client, as part of a systematic desensitization is the appropriate, if not mandated, clinical intervention. They tell us never to socialize with clients even though it is often impossible to avoid doing so in rural areas and in small communities. They tell us never to share a meal with a client even though an “anorexic lunch” can be part of a perfectly executed family system-based treatment plan. When it comes down to it, risk management can be also defined as attorneys’ advice overriding clinical judgment.
Risk management and the fear it induces effects not only mental health, but also our entire society. It is part of a bigger and more complex phenomenon: the American litigation explosion and the rights movement. Even though, as has been stated, litigation is rare in our profession, the mere possibility of such a consequence is daunting and affects us strongly. We have become a culture where everyone tramples everyone else in the fight for his or her rights and entitlements (Etzioni, 1987, Zur, 1994).
While accountability is an important civic quality, this lawsuit-happy culture, combined with the culture of victims, creates an atmosphere of dread that changes the way we do (or don’t do) business, play or healing. We all know about the playgrounds across the country that have been stripped of monkey bars, high slides and fun swings due to the thousands of lawsuits filed by parents after their children have ‘gotten hurt’ while playing. Ministers, teachers, and youth counselors avoid touching – especially children or women. People have actually sued McDonald’s for the obesity resulting from too many Big Macs! Litigation gone wild, indeed.
Flowing directly from this trend, fear-induced risk management has driven physicians to practice defensive medicine’ and to routinely order a huge number of diagnostic tests, such as lab and radiology investigations, not because they believe they are medically necessary but as part of their risk management strategy. The rationale is that the physician will not be accused of not having done everything in his power to rule out even the most unlikely diagnosis. Some estimate that doctors waste between $50 to $100 billion annually on defensive medicine. Shockingly, some also estimate that this is the amount that could buy health insurance for the 40 million uninsured Americans. While attorneys laugh all the way to the bank, our risk management-intoxicated, phobic culture ends up settling for inferior care of every kind; children are deprived of touch and opportunities for play; and spiraling medical costs continue to bankrupt the country.
One of the major concerns inherent in risk management is that fear induced, defensive behaviors, and the systematic avoidance of many behaviors have gradually stolen in through the back door to become the perceived standard of care in medicine and mental health (Zur, 2007a). The standard of care is not anything that can be found in a textbook; it evolves as more and more professionals adopt certain practices, which then achieve acceptance as the “standard” in the community. A prime example of how risk management affects the standard of care is the requirement that a woman chaperone be present during a gynecological pelvic exam. Besides providing some comfort to the female client, the chaperone’s primary role is to protect the physician from false accusation, criminal complaint or lawsuit. The witness also reduces the risk that misconduct may occur. Before chaperoning became part of the standard of care, some women preferred not to have such a witness, especially if they had a long, trusting relationship with their physician or if the physician was a woman. However, today, not having a witness is considered practicing below the standard of care. The danger that we face in mental health these days is that more and more risk management proscriptions may metamorphose into the standard of care.
Bear in mind, though, that risk management is neither part of the ethics codes nor part of any treatment standard. Risk management is merely a set of precautions advocated by malpractice insurance vendors and attorneys, supposedly to minimize the chances of being sued. When it comes to touch, good treatment and good risk management may sometimes call for mutually exclusive decisions regarding a given client. For example, it would probably be good risk management never to touch children and for male therapists never to touch female clients. Most of us would agree that such risk management advice is utter nonsense, since helping those in need is a fundamental ingredient of the psychotherapy professions. This example, like the case of the psychiatrist who refused to hold the grieving mother, illustrates the faulty logic and drawbacks of risk management and its single-minded devotion to avoiding lawsuits and its equally single-minded lack of regard for the primary goals of our work.
We have seen how, over time, a new standard of care insinuates itself into psychotherapy. This results in a continuous rising of the risk management bar as to what constitutes acceptable clinical behavior. Expert witnesses have often encountered testimonies by prominent experts that boldly and falsely asserted that touch, like gifts, extending a session beyond the allotted time or bartering always fail to meet the standard of care (Williams 1997). Similarly and dangerously, many licensing boards have uncritically accepted risk management recommendations as their guidelines. Paradoxically and ironically, as the bar is raised and more interventions seem frowned upon by the boards, courts and attorneys, there is increased likelihood that insurance companies and therapists will be sued or sanctioned. Risk management, without any doubt, has come to haunt the insurance companies, an unforeseen retribution for their shortsighted, cost-saving strategies. Sadly, it also impacts our profession negatively and often reduces our creativity and effectiveness, thus depriving our clients of the fullest measure of care.
The fear campaign by the insurance carriers, attorneys and many ethicists and “risk management experts” has too often succeeded in paralyzing therapists and forcing them to restrict themselves to rigid and constipated ways of relating to clients and avoiding any physical contact with their clients. As a result, clinical effectiveness is compromised. The danger that risk management poses to clinical effectiveness can be clearly seen in its injunction against touch which obviously has a significant negative effect on therapeutic alliance, the number one predictor of effective therapy (Lambert, 1991). We cannot think of any more effective ways to enhance therapeutic alliance then a reassuring or comforting hug, pat or hand holding. All of this may not be effective ‘risk management’, but it is basic good therapy.
Very regrettably, most professional organizations have jumped on the bandwagon and joined the fear campaign. They promote the practice of defensive medicine through their own risk management workshops and seminars. Some, as we see monthly, have given attorneys a regular column in their newsletters or journals where this paranoiac thinking is disseminated. As risk management becomes more prevalent, its effect is clearly seen on new therapists. In the numerous Ethics with Soul workshops I have conducted across the country, I (O.Z.) have noted that older (non-analytically oriented) therapists seem to be less concerned about risk management practices. The opposite is true of the new graduates. Alarmingly, through the fault of most graduate schools and their ethics professors, many of the newer therapists believe that risk management practices are part of the standard of care.
In summary, a risk managed practice may sound as if it adheres to practical or pragmatic advice but, in fact, it is a misnomer for a practice in which fear of attorneys and boards, rather than feeling, caring and intelligent clinical considerations, determine the course of therapy. As therapists, we are trained, hired and paid to provide the best care possible for clients. We are not paid to act defensively. This fear of board investigations and malpractice lawsuits pushes therapists to take protective measures. Consequently, we lower the quality of care for our clients.
Arnold Lazarus (1994) appropriately claims, “One of the worst professional or ethical violations is that of permitting current risk management principles to take precedence over humane interventions.” Similarly, one therapist asserts in his article on Counseling Today, “If you choose never to touch your clients, you probably will be relatively safe from accusations of impropriety, but you may also cheat your clients of one of the most powerful tools you have at your disposal” (Moffatt, 2017). Clinical interventions must be determined by empirically-based treatment plans, clinicians’ intuitive and creative sensitivities, and specific client factors, such as the client’s problems, situation, personality, degree of functionality, history, and culture — never by fear of boards and courts. We must remember that the therapeutic effect of touch has been scientifically and clinically proven. We must also remember that we are hired to help rather than being hired to practice risk management. Therefore we must touch clients when appropriate in a way that will help them grow and heal.
Slippery Slope Argument Around Touch
The term ‘slippery slope’ refers to the idea that failure to adhere to hands off, rigid standards, most commonly based on analytic and risk-management approaches, will undeniably harm clients, nullify therapeutic effectiveness and often leads to therapist-client sexual relationships. This fear-based view has been most dominant in the discussion of employing or incorporating touch in psychotherapy. It underlines most arguments against the use of physical touch by therapists. It asserts that a handshake, non-sexual hug or a re-assuring pat, are all just the first downhill steps towards inevitable deterioration, towards full- fledged sexual relationships.
The slippery slope process is described by Gabbard (1994) as follows: “. . . the crossing of one boundary without obvious catastrophic results (making) it easier to cross the next boundary” (p. 284). Pope (1990b), whose endorsement of the slippery slope idea has significantly contributed to its popularity, expresses a similar opinion: “. . . non-sexual dual relationships, while not unethical and harmful per se, foster sexual dual relationships” (p. 688). Strasburger et al. (1992) conclude, “Obviously, the best advice to therapists is not to start (down) the slippery slope, and to avoid boundary violations . . .” (p. 547-548). Sonne (1994) adds her version of the slippery slope: “With the blurring of the expected functions and responsibilities of the therapist and client comes the breakdown of the boundaries of the professional relationship itself” (p. 338).
Also in agreement is Simon (1991), who decrees that: “The boundary violation precursors of therapist-client sex can be as psychologically damaging as the actual sexual involvement itself” (p. 614). This poignant statement summarizes the opinion that the chance for exploitation and harm is reduced or nullified only by refraining from engaging in physical touch or any other boundary crossing. Many writers describe a long list of therapists’ behaviors (e. g. self-disclosure, hugs, home visits, socializing, longer sessions, lunching, exchanging gifts, walks, playing in recreational leagues) that they believe to be precursors to sexual dual relationships (Borys & Pope, 1989; Craig, 1991; Lakin, 1991; Pope, 1990a; Pope & Vasquez, 1998; Rutter, 1989; St. Germaine, 1996). Without doubt, touch tops this list.
The link between non-sexual touch and sexual violation is almost an epidemic in the field. In 1980, Holroyd & Brodsky titled their article regarding their survey, “Does touching clients lead to sexual intercourse?” Not surprisingly they also concluded that it “is difficult to determine where ‘non-erotic hugging, kissing, and affectionate touching’ leave off and ‘erotic contact’ begins” (p. 810). Similarly Pope and various colleagues (Pope & Bouhoutsos, 1986; Pope, et al., 1980; Pope & Tabachnick, 1994; Pope & Vasquez, 1998) have conducted several surveys that gave much more weight to the potential dangers of touching and how it is likely to lead to sexual intimacy rather than the more inquisitive balanced stance of looking at the positive and negative potentialities and unique complexities that touch introduces into therapy. Pope, Tabachnick and Keith-Spiegel (1987), citing several of their colleagues, state clearly that the attention to erotic contact in therapy has raised doubt about the “legitimacy and effects of ostensibly non-erotic physical contact” (p. 1001). Almost all ethics texts, like the widely used one by Koocher and Keith-Spiegel (1998), place the section of touch in the midst of chapter on sexual violation.
The slippery slope argument is grounded primarily in the assumption that touch or any boundary crossing, however trivial it may be, inevitably leads to sex and other boundary violations.
This argument is based on the finding that most therapists who were engaged in boundary violation had been engaged in boundary crossings prior to their engagement in boundary violations. However, to assert that a hug, self-disclosure, a home visit, or accepting a gift are actions likely to lead to sex is similar to saying that doctors’ visits cause death because most people see a doctor before they die (Zur, 2000). Lazarus (1994) calls this thinking “an extreme form of syllogistic reasoning” (p. 257). We learn in school that sequential statistical relationships (correlations) cannot simply be translated into causal connections.
The slippery slope notion that primarily touch, but in fact most boundary crossing, will inevitably end up with sex has been identified by Dineen (1996) as part of the more inclusive problem of psychotherapists’ sexualizing of all boundaries.
It is important to reiterate that whereas the analytic contingent underscores that crossing boundaries will nullify therapeutic effectiveness and hence cause harm, many other orientations have a different viewpoint. Behavioral, humanistic, group, family, existential, feminist or gestalt therapies at times stress the importance of tearing down interpersonal boundaries and strongly dispute that this will lead to exploitation and harm (Greenspan, 1995; Williams, 1997; Zur, 2000, 2001, 2007a, 2007b).
Top of Page
Summary
Touch is extremely important for health, healthy development and healing. The medicinal aspect of touch has been known and used since earliest recorded medical history, 25 centuries ago. Touch triggers a cascade of healing chemical responses including a decrease in stress hormones and an increase in serotonin and dopamine levels. Additionally, touch has been shown to increase the immune system’s cytotoxic capacity, thereby helping our body maintain its defenses and decreasing anxiety, depression, hyperactivity, inattention, stress hormones and cortisol levels.
In psychotherapy, there are many forms of touch. Among others, there are greeting, consoling, soothing, grounding, modeling and reassuring kinds of touch. In addition to the use of touch as an adjunct to psychotherapy there are several schools of thought, which are part of body psychotherapy orientations. These include Reichian, Radix and several other somatic therapies. Most of them use touch as a therapeutic technique. Erotic or sexual touch are always unethical and can be harmful.
There is a growing body of research that identifies the important clinical potential of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy. Clinically appropriate touch increases a clients’ sense of trust, comfort and ease with their therapists. As a result touch is highly effective in enhancing therapeutic alliance, which is the best predictor of positive therapeutic outcome.
The meaning of touch can only be understood within the context of who the client is, the therapeutic relationship, and the therapeutic setting. Accordingly, before employing touch, it is essential that the clinician consider unique treatment elements for each client including factors, such as culture, history, presenting problem, diagnosis, gender, history, etc. One must also consider the therapists’ education, training, theoretical orientations and comfort with non-sexual touch. Systematic touch should be employed in therapy only when it is well thought out and is likely to have positive clinical effects. Touch must be approached with caution with borderline or acutely paranoid clients. Special sensitivity is also required when working with people who tend to sexualize relationships and/or have been abused, molested or raped. There is also a growing body of knowledge that shows the damage done by the systematic and rigid avoidance of all forms of touch in therapy.
The field of psychotherapy appears to be becoming increasingly polarized regarding the use of touch in psychotherapy. At the extreme end of one pole are the proponents of risk management practices and the “slippery slope” ideology, whereby the simplest form of social touch− a handshake, a pat on the back, a comforting hug− are seen as the first dangerous, incremental steps in the direction of the extreme misuse of power that will inevitably and unalterably result in sexual violation and psychological injury of the client. Many therapists are motivated by fear of the appearance of wrongdoing and therefore avoid all forms of touch. For theoretical reasons, psychoanalytically oriented therapists are opposed to any form of touch. At the opposite end of the pole are those therapists from traditions which have historically valued touch as a congruent aspect of the therapeutic bond and as an acceptable tool in assisting a client to reach therapeutic goals. These therapists train in traditions that value and address the complex issues involved in utilizing touch with multiple populations while also addressing individual client’s idiosyncrasies.
Unfortunately, due to the absence of attention to touch in most training programs, clinical supervision, outcome research and testing, most therapists do not think critically about incorporating the use of touch into treatment plans. When they do touch they employ “touch but don’t talk”. Some touch in response to their own unexamined neurotic needs. Of course, these are the practitioners who are most likely to cause real injury to their clients. Additionally, the possible negative consequences of never touching our clients must be taken into consideration. Other therapists intuitively touch in a way that is supportive of the therapeutic bond, triggering the release of natural anti-depressant and stress reducing chemicals, increasing a client’s sense of self-esteem, and helping the client move toward therapeutic goals. A professional, responsible solution would be for all therapists to receive education and training that will allow the powerful therapeutic aspects of touch to be used responsibly and discriminatively in the care of their clients.
In summary, touch is important for healthy development and healing. Looking critically, not paradoxically, at the issue of touch in therapy, one can easily see the fallacy and danger of the slippery slope ideology, risk management practices or rigid adherence to analytic guidelines. The decision to touch or not to touch is dependent upon the client’s personality, diagnosis, symptoms, culture, gender, history, etc., as well as the context of the relationship and the training and awareness of the therapist. Touching inappropriately can be damaging, as can be the rigid and indiscriminant avoidance of touch. Touch, is one of the most basic forms of human relatedness, and has immense importance and positive potential for inclusion in psychotherapy.
Top of Page
General Points And Ethical And Clinical Guidelines About Touch In Psychotherapy
THE GENERAL SIGNIFICANCE OF TOUCH
- Touch is one of the most essential elements of human development: a form of communication, critical for healthy development and one of the most significant healing forces.
- In his seminal work, Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin, Ashley Montagu (1971) brought together a great array of studies demonstrating the significant role of physical touch in human development.
- The effects of touch deficiencies can have lifelong serious negative ramifications.
- Bowlby and Harlow, among many others, concluded that touch, rather than feeding, bonds infant to caregiver.
- Touch has a high degree of cultural relativity. People of Anglo-Saxon origin place low on a continuum of touch while those of Latin, Mediterranean and third world ancestry place on the high end.
- The general western culture and its emphasis on autonomy, independence, separateness and privacy have resulted in restricting interpersonal physical touch to a minimum. America is a low touch culture.
- In Western society, sex, love, power and dominance are dangerously confused.
- Americans tend to sexualize or infantilize the meaning of touch and as a result tend to avoid touch. Watson, parenting expert of the early 1900’s, cautioned mothers not to sexualize their infants by kissing or hugging them affectionately.
TOUCH AND HEALING
- The medicinal aspect of touch has been known and utilized since earliest recorded medical history, 25 centuries ago.
- Touch unleashes a stream of healing chemical responses including a decrease in stress hormones and an increase in serotonin and dopamine levels.
- Touch increases the immune system’s cytotoxic capacity thereby helping our body maintain its defenses.
- Massage has been shown to decrease anxiety, depression, hyperactivity, inattention, stress hormones and cortisol levels.
- Massaged babies are more sociable and more easily soothed than babies who have not been massaged.
TYPES OF TOUCH IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
- Ritualistic or socially accepted gestures
- Conversational Marker
- Consoling or reassuring
- Playful touch
- Grounding or reorienting
- Task-Oriented
- Corrective experience
- Instructional or modeling
- Celebratory or congratulatory
- Experiential
- Referential
- Inadvertent
- Preventing someone from hurting self or others
- Self-defense
- Therapeutic intervention – A bodytherapy medical technique
- Inappropriate, unethical and probably illegal forms of touch include sexual, hostile-violent and punishing touch.
SOURCES OF THE PROHIBITION OF TOUCH IN THERAPY
- The general western culture and its emphasis on autonomy, independence, separateness and privacy.
- The cultural tendency in the USA to sexualize most forms of touch.
- The traditional dualistic Western mind-body or mental-physical split.
- Homophobia.
- Some fundamentalist religious denominations that have a highly restrictive view of all forms of touch.
- The litigious culture and the resulting risk management and defensive medicine practices.
- Psychoanalysis and its emphasis on neutrality, distance and rigid boundaries.
- Those feminist scholars who assert that any touch by male therapists of female patients is disempowering and injuring to the women.
- The fear-based, illogical slippery slope idea that non-sexual touch inevitably leads to sexual exploitation.
- The more recent crisis in the clergy and the not too distant daycare hysteria in regard to sexual exploitation.
ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS OF NON-SEXUAL TOUCH IN THERAPY
- Touch in therapy is not inherently unethical.
- None of the professional organizations code of ethics (i.e., APA, ApA, ACA, NASW, CAMFT) view touch as unethical.
- Touch should be employed in therapy when it is likely to have positive therapeutic effect.
- Practicing risk management by rigidly avoiding touch is unethical. Therapists are not paid to protect themselves; they are hired to help, heal, support, etc.
- Avoiding touch in therapy on account of fear of boards or attorneys is unethical.
- Rigidly withholding touch from children and other clients who can benefit from it, such as those who are anxious, dissociative, grieving or terminally ill can be harming and therefore unethical.
- Sexual, erotic or violent touch in therapy is always unethical.
- Stopping therapy in order to engage in sexual touch or sexual relationships is unethical and often illegal.
- Ethical touch is the touch that is employed with consideration to the context of the therapeutic relationship and with sensitivity to clients’ variables, such as gender, culture, history, diagnosis, etc.
- Seeking ethical consultation is important in complex and sensitive cases.
- Ethical therapists should thoroughly process their feelings, attitudes and thoughts regarding touch in general and the often unavoidable attraction to particular clients.
- Critical thinking and thorough ethical-decision making are most important processes preceding the ethical use of touch in therapy.
- Documentation of type, frequency and rationale of extensive touch is an important aspect of ethical practice.
CLINICAL CONSIDERATIONS FOR TOUCH IN PSYCHOTHERAPY
- The meaning of touch can only be understood within the context of who the patient is, the therapeutic relationship, and the therapeutic setting.
- Touch, like any other therapists’ behavior and interventions should be employed if they are likely to help clients.
- Touch increases therapeutic alliance, the factor found to be the best predictor of therapeutic outcome.
- Touch can help therapists to provide real or symbolic contact and nurturance, to facilitate access to, exploration of, and resolution of emotional experiences, to provide containment, and to restore significant and healthy dimensions in relationships.
- Clinically appropriate touch must be employed with sensitivity to clients’ variables, such as history, gender, culture, diagnosis, etc.
- Sensitive, attuned touch gets etched into our developing neural pathways enabling us to feel of value, and to connect emotionally with others. As such, touch can be a powerful method of healing.
- Language never completely supersedes the more primitive form of communication, physical touch. As such it can have a significant therapeutic value.
- The unduly restrictive analytic, risk management or defensive medicine emphasis on rigid and inflexible boundaries and the mandate to avoid touch interferes with human relatedness and sound clinical judgment.
- Due to the absence of attention to touch in most training programs, clinical supervision, research and testing, the majority of therapists tend not to incorporate the use of touch in therapy.
- Fear, misguided beliefs and lack of training often lead to therapists employing an approach of “touch but don’t talk.”
- Touch that is inappropriate, sexual, cold or abusive can be harmful.
- Traumatic memories are encoded in our sensorimotor system as kinesthetic sensations and images, while the linguistic encoding of memory is suppressed. Therefore, appropriate touch can have a significant therapeutic value. A variety of studies have shown that specific forms of therapeutic touch appear to be effective in the treatment strategy for patients suffering from trauma including women in domestic violence situations (Saint Arnault, 2015), children suffering from PTSD (Ahmad, 2007), and combat-exposed active duty soldiers suffering from post-traumatic stress disorder (Jain et al., 2012).
- Disturbances in non-verbal communication are more severe and often longer lasting than disturbances in verbal language. Using touch in therapy may be the only way to heal some of these disturbances.
- To disregard all physical contact between therapist and client may deter or limit psychological growth.
GUIDELINES FOR CLINICAL AND ETHICAL TOUCH IN THERAPY
- Touch should be employed in therapy if it is likely to be helpful and clinically effective.
- Avoiding touch due to fear of boards and attorneys is unethical and a betrayal of our clinical commitment to aid clients.
- Touch in therapy must always be employed with full consideration to the context of therapy and clients’ factors, such as presenting problems and symptoms, personal touch and sexual history, ability to differentiate types of touch, the clients level of ability to assertively identify and protect his or her boundaries as well as the gender, and cultural influences of both the client and the therapist.
- Touch should be used according to the therapists training and competence.
- Extensive touch should be incorporated into the written treatment planning.
- The decision to touch should include a thorough deliberation of the client’s potential perception and interpretation of touch.
- Therapists must be particularly careful to structure a foundation of client safety and empowerment before using touch.
- Factors that are associated with congruence are clarity regarding boundaries, the patient’s perception of being in control of the physical contact, and the patient’s perception that the touch is for his/her benefit rather than the therapists.
- The therapist should state clearly that there will be no sexual contact and to be clear about the process and type of touch that will be used.
- Permission to touch should be obtained from clients in a form of a written consent if therapy involves extensive use of touch, such as is utilized in some forms of body psychotherapy.
- Touch is usually contraindicated for clients who are highly paranoid, actively hostile or aggressive, highly sexualized or who implicitly or explicitly demand touch.
- Special care should be taken in the use of touch with people who have experienced assault, neglect, attachment difficulties, rape, molestation, sexual addictions, eating disorders, and intimacy issues.
- Therapists should not avoid touch out of fear of boards, attorneys or dread of litigation. Therapists are paid to provide the best care for their clients not to practice risk management.
- Consultation is recommended in complex cases.
- Therapists have a responsibility to explore their personal issues regarding touch and to seek education and consultation regarding the appropriate use of touch in psychotherapy.
Top of Page
References
Abbey, A., & Melby, C. (1986). The effects of non-verbal cues on gender differences in perceptions of sexual intent. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 15(5-6), 283-298. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00288318
Ahmad, A., Larsson, B., & Sundelin-Wahlsten, V. (2007). EMDR treatment for children with PTSD: Results of a randomized controlled trial. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 61(5), 349-354. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039480701643464
Ainsworth, M. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Allen, J. G. (1995). Coping with trauma: A guide to self-understanding. American Psychiatric Press, Inc.
Alvarez, M.J., Fernandez, D., Gomez-Salgado, J., Rodriguez-Gonzalez, D., Roson, M., & Lapena, S. (2017). The effects of massage therapy hospitalized preterm neonate: A systematic review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 69, 119-136. https://doi.org.10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2017.02.009
Alvarez, M.J., Rodriguez-Gonzalez, D., Roson, M., Lapena, S., Gomez-Salgado, J., & Fernandez-Garcia, D. (2019). Effects of massage therapy and kinesitherapy to develop hospitalized preterm infant’s anthropometry: A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 46, 86-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2019.03.015
Alyn, J. H. (1988). The politics of touch in therapy: A response to Willison & Masson. Journal of Counseling & Development, 66(9), 432-433. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1988.tb00906.x
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. (2012, July 30). Corporal punishment. https://www.aacap.org/aacap/Policy_Statements/2012/Policy_Statement_on_Corporal_Punishment.aspx
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2018, November 5). American Academy of Pediatrics updates policy on corporal punishment. https://www.aap.org/en-us/about-the-aap/aap-press-room/aap-press-room-media-center/Pages/AAP-Updates-Policy-on-Corporal-Punishment-.aspx
American Association for Marriage and Family Therapy. (2015). AAMFT Code of ethics. https://www.aamft.org/Legal_Ethics/Code_of_Ethics.aspx
American Counseling Association. (2014). ACA Code of ethics. https://www.counseling.org/resources/aca-code-of-ethics.pdf
American Psychological Association. (2012). Understanding psychotherapy and how it works. http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/understanding-psychotherapy
American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. https://www.apa.org/ethics/code/ethics-code-2017.pdf
Anisfeld, E., Casper, V., Nozyce, M., & Cunningham, M. (1990). Does infant carrying promote attachment? An experimental study of the effects of increased physical contact on the development of attachment. Child Development, 61(5), 1617-1627. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02888.x
Aponte, J. F., & Wohl, J. (2000). Psychological intervention and cultural diversity. Allyn & Bacon.
Balint, M. (1968). The basic fault: Therapeutic aspects of regression. Tavistock.
Ball, A. (2002). Taboo or not taboo: Reflections on physical touch in psychoanalysis & somatic psychotherapy. Psychoz Publications.
Bar-Levav, R. (1998). A rationale for physical touching in psychotherapy. In E. W. L. Smith, P. R. Clance, & S. Imes (Eds.), Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 52-55). Guilford Press.
Bar-Levav, R. (1988). Thinking in the shadow of feelings: A new understanding of the hidden forces that shape individuals and societies. Simon & Schuster.
Barnett, L. (2005). Keep in touch: The importance of touch in infant development. Infant Observation, 8(2), 115-123. https://doi.org/10.1080/13698030500171530
Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. Dutton.
Bauer, B. (1977). Tactile-sensitive behavior in hyperactive and non-hyperactive children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 31(7), 447-453.
Bellehumeur, C. R., & Chambers, J. (2017). Contributions of sensory anthropology and Durand’s anthropology to the symbolic study of touch and the understanding of boundaries in psychotherapy. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 52-68). University of Ottawa Press. https://www.jstor.org/stable/j.ctv5vdcvd
Berendsen, P. (2017). The intervention of touch in psychotherapy and trauma treatment. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 85-105). University of Ottawa Press. https://ruor.uottawa.ca/bitstream/10393/37596/1/9780776627564_WEB.pdf
Bersoff, D. N. (Ed.). (1999). Ethical conflicts in psychology (2nd ed.). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10329-000
Black, S. C. (2017). To cross or not to cross: Ethical boundaries in psychological practice. Journal of the Australian and New Zealand Student Service Association, 25(1), 1-18. file:///C:/Users/M/Downloads/1339-to-cross-or-not-to-cross-ethical-boundaries-in-psychological-practice.pdf
Blatner, A. (2002). About non-verbal communications: General considerations. https://www.blatner.com/adam/level2/nverb1.htm
Bonitz, V. (2008). Use of physical touch in the “talking cure”: A journey to the outskirts of psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 45(3), 391-404. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013311
Borys, D. S. (1992). Nonsexual dual relationships. In L. VandeCreek, S. Knapp, & T. L. Jackson (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book (Vol. 11, pp. 443-454). Professional Resource Exchange.
Borys, D. S. (1994). Maintaining therapeutic boundaries: The motive is therapeutic effectiveness, not defensive practice. Ethics & Behavior, 4(3), 267-273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327019eb0403_12
Borys, D. S., & Pope, K. S. (1989). Dual relationships between therapist and client: A national study of psychologists, psychiatrists, and social workers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 20(5), 283-293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.20.5.283
Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health: A report prepared on behalf of the World Health Organization as a contribution to the United Nations programme for the welfare of homeless children. World Health Organization.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment: Separation & loss (Vol. 1).. Basic Books.
Bremner, J. (2002). Does stress damage the brain?: Understanding trauma-related disorders from a mind-body perspective. Norton.
Bremner, A.J., & Spence, C. (2017). Chapter 7: The development of tactile perception. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 52, 227-268. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2016.12.002
Brodsky, A. M. (1985). Sex between therapists and patients: Ethical gray areas. Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 3(1), 57-62. https://doi.org/10.1300/J294v03n01_07
Brody, V. A. (1978). Developmental play: A relationship-focused program for children. Child Welfare: Journal of Policy, Practice, and Program, 57(9), 591-599.
Brown, L. S. (1985). Power, responsibility, boundaries: Ethical concerns for the lesbian feminist therapist. Lesbian Ethics, 1(3), 30-45.
Brown, L. S. (1994). Boundaries in feminist therapy: A conceptual formulation. In N. K. Gartrell (Ed.), Bringing ethics alive: Feminist ethics in psychotherapy practice (pp. 29-38). The Haworth Press.
Burkholder, D., Toth, M., Feisthamel, K., & Britton, P. (2010) Faculty and student curricular experiences of nonerotic touch in counseling. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 32(2), 168-185. https://doi.org/10.17744/mehc.32.2.b443p51w6x140555
Caldwell, C. (1997). Getting in touch: The guide to new body-centered therapies. Quest Books.
Cascio, C. J., Moore, D., & McGlone, F. (2019). Social touch and human development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 5-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009
Chen, S.C., Yu, B.Y., Suen, L.K., Yu, J., Ho, F.Y., Yang, J.J., & Yeung, W.F. (2019). Massage therapy for the treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 42, 389-399. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2018.12.011
Clay, V. S. (1966). The effect of culture on mother-child tactile communication (Doctoral dissertation). Teachers College, Columbia University.
Cleveland, L., Hill, C., Pulse, W. Dicioccio, H., Field, T., & White-Traut, R. (2017). Systematic review of skin-to-skin care for full-term, healthy newborns. Journal of Obstetric and Gynecological Nursing, 46, 857-868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jogn.2017.08.005
Cohen, S. S. (1987). The magic of touch. Harper & Row.
Comas-Diaz, L., & Green, B. (Eds.). (1994). Women of color: Integrating ethnic and gender identities in psychotherapy. Guilford Press.
Corey, G., Corey, A. S., & Callanan, P. (1984). Issues and ethics in the helping professions. Brooks/Cole.
Cornell, W. F. (1997). Touch and boundaries in Transactional Analysis: Ethical and transferential considerations. Transactional Analysis Journal, 27(1), 30-37. https://doi.org/10.1177/036215379702700108
Courtney, J. A., & Siu, A. F. Y. (2018). Practitioner experiences of touch in working with children in play therapy. International Journal of Play Therapy, 27(2), 92-102. https://doi.org/10.1037/pla0000064
Courtois, C. (1998). Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors in therapy. Norton.
Cowen, E. L., Weissberg, R. P., & Lotyczewski, B. S. (1983). Physical contact in interactions between clinicians and young children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(1), 132-138. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.51.1.132
Craig, J. D. (1991). Preventing dual relationships in pastoral counseling. Counseling and Values, 36(1), 49-54. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-007X.1991.tb00778.x
Davis, I., Rovers, M., & Petrella, C. (2017). Touch deprivation and counselling as healing touch. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 13-31). University of Ottawa Press. https://ruor.uottawa.ca/bitstream/10393/37596/1/9780776627564_WEB.pdf
Dineen, T. (1996). Manufacturing victims: What the psychology industry is doing to people. Robert Davies Publishing.
Downey, D. L. (2001). Therapeutic touch in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 36(1), 35-38.
Dunne, C., Bruggen, P., & O’Brian, C. (1982). Touch and action in group therapy of younger adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 5(1), 31-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-1971(82)80016-X
Durana, C. (1998). The use of touch in psychotherapy: Ethical and clinical guidelines. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 35(2), 269-280. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087817
Edwards, D. J. (1984). The experience of interpersonal touch during a personal growth program: A factor analytic approach. Human Relations, 37(9), 769-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872678403700904
Eiden, B. (1998). The use of touch in psychotherapy. Self & Society 26(2), 3-8. https://doi.org/10.1080/03060497.1998.11085839
Epstein, R. S., & Simon, R. I. (1990). The Exploitation Index: An early warning indicator of boundary violations in psychotherapy. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 54(4), 450-465.
Etzioni, A. (1991). A responsive society: Collected essays on guiding deliberate social change. Jossey-Bass, Inc.
Fagan, J. (1998). Thoughts on using touch in psychotherapy. In E. W. Smith, P. R. Clance, & S. Imes (Eds.), Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 145-152). Guilford Press.
Field, T. (1995). Infant massage therapy. In T. M. Field (Ed.), Touch in early development (pp. 105-114). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Field, T. (1998). Massage therapy effects. American Psychologist, 53(12), 1270-1281. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.12.1270
Field, T. (2005). Touch deprivation and aggression against self among adolescents. In D. M. Stoff & E. J. Susman (Eds.), Developmental psychobiology of aggression (pp. 117-140). Cambridge University Press.
Field, T. (2014). Touch (2nd ed.). MIT Press.
Field, T., Grizzle, N., Scafidi, F., Abrams, S., Richardson, S., Kuhn, C., & Schanberg, S. (1996). Massage therapy for infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behavior & Development, 19(1), 107-112. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90048-X
Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Hart, S., Quintino, O., Drose, L., Field, T., Kuhn, C., & Schanberg, S. (1997). Sexual abuse effects are lessened by massage therapy, Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 1(2), 65-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1360-8592(97)80002-2
Field, T., Lasko, D., Mundy, P., Henteleff, T., Kabat, S., Talpins, S., & Dowling, M. (1997). Autistic children’s attentiveness and responsivity improve after touch therapy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 333-338. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1025858600220
Field, T., Morrow, C., Valdeon, C., Larson, S., Kuhn, C., & Schanberg, S. (1992). Massage reduces anxiety in child and adolescent psychiatric patients. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(1), 125-131. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199201000-00019
Field, T., Seligman, S., Scafidi, F., & Schanberg, S. (1996). Alleviating posttraumatic stress in children following Hurricane Andrew. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 37-50. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(96)90004-0
Field, T., Schanberg, S., Kuhn, C., Fierro, K., Henteleff, T., Mueller, C., Yando, R., Shaw, S., & Burman, I. (1998). Bulimic adolescents benefit from massage therapy. Adolescence, 33(131), 555-563.
Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Wormuth, B. K., Vanderminden, J., & Hamby, S. (2019). Corporal punishment: Current rates from a national survey. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28, 1991-1997. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01426-4
Fosshage, J. L. (2000). The meanings of touch in psychoanalysis: A time for reassessment. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 20(1), 21-43. https://doi.org/10.1080/07351692009348873
Frank, J. D. (1970). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy. Schocken Books.
Frank, L. K. (1957). Tactile communication. General Psychological Monographs, 56, 209-255.
Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression: An evolutionary view. Academic Press.
Gabbard, G. O. (1994). Teetering on the precipice: A commentary on Lazarus’s “How certain boundaries and ethics diminish therapeutic effectiveness.” Ethics & Behavior, 4(3), 283-286. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327019eb0403_14
Geib, P. (1982). The experience of nonerotic physical contact in traditional psychotherapy. In E. W. L. Smith, P. R. Clance, & S. Imes (Eds.) Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 109-126). Guilford Press.
Gendlin, E. T. (1982). Focusing (2nd ed.). Bantam Books.
Givens, D. G. (2004). Center for non-verbal studies. http://members.aol.com/non-verbal2/index.htm.
Glickauf-Hughes, C., & Clance, P. R. (1998). An individualized and interactive object relations perspective on the use of touch in psychotherapy. In W. L. Smith, P. R. Clance, & S. Imes (Eds.), Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 153-169). Guilford Press.
Gottlieb, M. C. (1993). Avoiding exploitive dual relationships: A decision-making model. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 30(1), 41-48. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.30.1.41
Greene, B. (Ed.). (1997). Psychological perspectives on lesbian and gay issues: Vol. 3. Ethnic and cultural diversity among lesbians and gay men. Sage Publications.
Greenspan, M. (1995, July/August). Out of bounds. Common Boundary Magazine, 51-56.
Guindon, M., Packard, R., & Charron, N. (2017). The ethics of touch in the helping relationships. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 213-235). University of Ottawa Press. https://ruor.uottawa.ca/bitstream/10393/37596/1/9780776627564_WEB.pdf
Gurman, A. (1977). The patient’s perception of the therapeutic relationship. In A. Gurman & A. Razin (Eds.), Effective psychotherapy: A handbook of research (pp. 503-543). Pergamon Press.
Gutheil, T. G., & Gabbard, G. O. (1993). The concept of boundaries in clinical practice: Theoretical and risk-management dimensions. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(2), 188-196. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.150.2.188
Halbrook, B., & Duplechin, R. (1994). Rethinking touch in psychotherapy: Guidelines for practitioners. Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 13(3), 43-53.
Hall, J. (1984). Non-verbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Johns Hopkins University Press.
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13(12), 673-685. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047884
Harlow, H. F. (1971). Learning to love. Albion.
Harrison, C., Jones, R. S.P., & Huws, J. C. (2012). “We’re people who don’t touch”: Exploring clinical psychologists’ perspectives on their use of touch in therapy. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 25(3), 277-287. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2012.671595
Hartley, D., & Strupp, H. (1983). The therapeutic alliance: Its relationship to outcome in brief psychotherapy. In J. Masling (Ed.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories (Vol. 1, pp. 1-37). Erlbaum.
Helfer, R. E. (1978). Childhood comes first: A crash course in childhood for adults. (n.p.).
Heller, S. (1997). The vital touch. Henry Holt & Company.
Heller, S. (1999, Sept). Touch taboos. Mothering, 96:44 et seq.
Help, A. (2018, December 22). Is there a touch taboo in psychotherapy?: Five arguments in favor of the use of touch in therapy. NeuroAffective Touch. https://neuroaffectivetouch.com/natouch-blog/is-there-a-touch-taboo-in-psychotherapy/
Herlihy, B., & Corey, G. (1992). Dual relationships in counseling. American Association for Counseling and Development.
Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. The aftermath of violence-from domestic abuse to political terror. Basic Books.
Hernandez-Reif, M., Field, T., Krasnegor, J., Hossain, Z., Theakston, H., & Burman, I. (2000). High blood pressure and associated symptoms were reduced by massage therapy. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 4(1), 31-38. https://doi.org/10.1054/jbmt.1999.0129
Hilton, R. (1997). Touching in therapy. In L. E. Hedges, R. Hilton, V. W. Hilton, O. Brandf Caudill, Jr. (Eds.). Therapists at risk: Perils of the intimacy of the therapeutic relationships. Jason Aronson, Inc.
Hoffman, D. (2016). Touch-inclusive therapy with survivors of sexual trauma: A phenomenological investigation into the experience of touch in sexual trauma therapy. [Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Chicago School of Professional Psychology].
Hollender, M. H., & Mercer, A. J. (1976). Wish to be held and wish to hold in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(1), 49-51. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770010029005
Hollinger, L. (1986). Communicating with the elderly. Journal of Gerentological Nursing, 12(3), 8-13.
Holroyd, J. C., & Brodsky, A. M. (1977). Psychologists’ attitudes and practices regarding erotic and nonerotic physical contact with patients. American Psychologist, 32(10), 843-849. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.10.843
Holroyd, J. C., & Brodsky, A. M. (1980). Does touching patients lead to sexual intercourse? Professional Psychology, 11(5), 807-811. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.11.5.807
Holub, E. A., & Lee, S. S. (1990). Therapists’ use of nonerotic physical contact: Ethical concerns. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21(2), 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.21.2.115
Horton, J. A., Clance, P. R., Sterk-Elifson, C., & Emshoff, J. (1995). Touch in psychotherapy: A survey of patients’ experiences. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 32(3), 443-457. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.32.3.443
Hovdestad, W. E., & Kristiansen, C. M. (1996). Mind meets body: On the nature of recovered memories of trauma. Women & Therapy, 19(1), 31-45. https://doi.org/10.1300/J015v19n01_04
Hunter, M., & Struve, J. (1998). The ethical use of touch in psychotherapy. Sage Publications.
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. (1998). Childhood trauma remembered: A report on the current scientific knowledge base and its applications. Author.
Ironson, G., Field, T., Scafidi, F., Hashimoto, M., Kumar, M., Kumar, A., Price, A., Goncalves, A., Burman, I., Tetenman, C., Patarca, R., & Fletcher, M. A. (1996). Massage therapy is associated with enhancement of the immune system’s cytotoxic capacity. International Journal of Neuroscience, 84(1-4), 205-218. https://doi.org/10.3109/00207459608987266
Jain, S., McMahon, G. F., Hasen, P., Kozub, M. P., Porter, V., King, R., & Guarneri, E. M. (2012). Healing touch with guided imagery for PTSD in returning active duty military: A randomized controlled trial. Military Medicine, 177(9), 1015-1021. https://doi.org/10.7205/milmed-d-11-00290
Jakubiak, B. K., & Feeney, B. C. (2016). Affectionate touch to promote relational, psychological, and physical well-being in adulthood: A theoretical model and review of the research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 228-252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316650307
Jean, A. D. L., Stack, D. M., & Arnold, S. (2014). Investigating maternal touch and infants’ self-regulatory behaviours during a modified face-to-face still-face with touch procedure. Infant and Child Development, 23(6), 557–574. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1870
Johnson, D. (1983). Body. Beacon Press.
Johnson, D., & Grand, I. J. (Eds.). (1998). The body in psychotherapy: Inquiries in somatic psychology. North Atlantic Books.
Jones, S. E. (1994). The right touch: Understanding and using the language of physical contact. Hampton Press.
Jourard, S. M. (1966). An exploratory study of body accessibility. British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 5(3), 221-231. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1966.tb00978.x
Kagle, J. D., & Geilbelhausen, P. N. (1994). Dual relationships and professional boundaries. Social Work, 39(2), 213-220. https://doi.org/10.1093/sw/39.2.213
Karbelnig, A. (2000). Physical contact between psychotherapist and patient: Ethical, legal, and psychoanalytical considerations. The California Psychologist, 33(6), 32-34.
Katherine, A. (1993). Boundaries: Where you end and I begin. Fireside/Parkside.
Katsurada, E. (2012). The relationship between parental physical affection and child physical aggression among Japanese preschoolers. Child Studies in Diverse Contexts, 2(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.5723/csdc.2012.2.1.001
Keith-Spiegel, P., & Koocher, G. P. (1985). Ethics in psychology: Professional standards and cases. Random House.
Keleman, S. (1981). Your body speaks its mind. Center Press.
Keleman, S. (1987). Embodying experience: Forming a personal life. Center Press.
Kerr, F., Wiechula, R., Feo, R., Schultz, T., & Kitson, A. (2019). Neurophysiology of human touch and eye gaze in therapeutic relationships and healing: A scoping review. JBI Database of Systematic Reviews and Implementation Reports, 17(2), 209-247. https://doi.org/ 10.11124/JBISRIR-2017-003549
Kitchener, K. S. (1988). Dual role relationships: What makes them so problematic? Journal of Counseling & Development, 67(4), 217-221. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1988.tb02586.x
Klaus, M. (2009). Commentary: An early, short, and useful sensitive period in the human infant. Birth: Issues in Perinatal Care, 36(2), 110-112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-536X.2009.00315.x
Klaus, M., & Kennel, J. (1976). Maternal-infant bonding. C.V. Mosby Co.
Knapp, M. L., & Hall, J. A. (2001). Non-verbal communication in human interaction (5th ed.). Wadsworth Publishing.
Koocher, G. P., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (1998). Ethics in psychology: Professional standards and cases (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
Kurtz, R. (1990). Body-centered psychotherapy: The Hakomi method. Life Rhythm.
Lakin, M. (1991). Psychology practitioner guidebooks: Coping with ethical dilemmas in psychotherapy. Pergamon Press.
Lamb, M. E., Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W., & Charnov, E. L. (Eds.). (1985). Infant-mother attachment: The origins and developmental significance of individual differences in strange situation behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lambert, M. J. (1991). Introduction to psychotherapy research. In L. E. Beutler & M. Crago (Eds.), Psychotherapy research: An international review of programmatic studies (pp. 1-11). American Psychological Association.
Lambert, M. J. (1992). Implications of outcome research for psychotherapy integration. In J. C. Norcross & M. R. Goldfried (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy integration (pp. 94-129). Basic Books.
Langs, R. (1976). The therapeutic relationship and deviations in technique. In R. J. Langs (Ed.), International journal of psychoanalytic psychotherapy: Vol. 4 (pp. 106-141). Jason Aronson.
Langs, R. (1988). A primer of psychotherapy. Gardner Press.
LaPierre, A. (2012). Healing touch: Honoring the somatic dimension of psyche. Available from http://cellularbalance.com/article_listings.html
Lawry, S. (1998). Touch and clients who have been sexually abused. In W. L. Smith, P. R. Clance, & S. Imes (Eds.), Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 201-210). Guilford Press.
Lazarus, A. A. (1994). How certain boundaries and ethics diminish therapeutic effectiveness. Ethics and Behavior, 4(3), 255-261. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327019eb0403_10
Lazarus, A. A., & Zur, O. (Eds.). (2002). Dual relationships and psychotherapy. Springer.
Levine, P. A., & Frederick, A. (1997). Waking the tiger: Healing trauma: The innate capacity to transform overwhelming experiences. North Atlantic Books.
Levitan, A. A., & Johnson, J. M. (1986). The role of touch in healing and hypnotherapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 28(4), 218-223. https://doi.org/10.1080/00029157.1986.10402657
Little, M. I. (1990). Psychotic anxieties and containment: A personal record of an analysis with Winnicott. Jason Aronson.
Lowen, A. (1958). Physical dynamics of character structure. Greenwood.
Lowen, A. (1976). Bioenergetics. Penguin.
Marziali, E. (1984). Three viewpoints on the therapeutic alliance: Similarities, differences, and associations with psychotherapy outcome. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172(7), 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-198407000-00008
McGrane, C. (2019). An exploration on the use of touch in the therapeutic relationship. [Master’s thesis, Dublin Business School]. https://esource.dbs.ie/bitstream/handle/10788/3761/hdip_mcgrane_c_2019.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
McGuirk, J. (2012). The place of touch in counselling and psychotherapy and the potential for healing within the therapeutic relationship. Inside Out. Retrieved from http://iahip.org/inside-out/issue-68-autumn-2012/the-place-of-touch-in-counselling-and-psychotherapy-and-the-potential-for-healing-within-the-therapeutic-relationship
McLaughlin, J. T. (1995). Touching limits in the analytic dyad. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 64(3), 443-465. https://doi.org/10.1080/21674086.1995.11927460
McNeely, D. A. (1987). Touching: Body therapy and depth psychology. Inner City Books.
Mead, M. (1955). Cited in D. M. Schneider & G. C. Homans, “Kinship terminology and the American kinship system” American Anthropologist, 57(6), 1194-1208. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1955.57.6.02a00100
Mead, M. (1967). Male and female. William Morrow (pp. 83-84).
Mehrabian, A. (1971). The silent language: Implicit communication of emotions and attitudes. Wadsworth Publishing Co.
Menehan, K. (1996). The perinatal bonding project: Infant massage to reduce child abuse. Massage Magazine. Spokane, WA.
Menninger, K. (1958). Menninger Clinic monograph series: Theory of psychoanalytic technique. Basic Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/10843-000
Menninger, K. (1961). Theory of psychoanalytic technique. Science Editions.
Milakovitch, J. C. (1993). Touching in psychotherapy: The differences between therapists who touch and those who do not. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(6-B), 3347.
Miller, L. A. (1979). An explanation of therapeutic touch: Using the science of unitary man. Nursing Forum, 18(3), 278-287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6198.1979.tb00680.x
Mindell, A. (1983). Dreambody: The body’s role in revealing the self. Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Mintz, N. L. (1971). Patient fees and psychotherapeutic transactions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030483
Mitchell, G. (1979). Behavioral sex differences in nonhuman primates. Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Moffatt, G. K. (2017, March 7). The healing language of appropriate touch. Counseling Today: Member Insights. https://ct.counseling.org/2017/03/healing-language-appropriate-touch/#
Montagu, A. (1971). Touching: The human significance of the skin. Columbia University Press.
Montagu, A. (1986). Touching: The human significance of the skin (3rd ed.). Harper & Row.
Moszkowski, R. J., Stack, D. M., Girouard, N., Field, T. M., Hernandez-Reif, M., & Diego, M. (2009). Touching behaviors of infants of depressed mothers during normal and perturbed interactions. Infant Behavior & Development, 32(2), 183-194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.009
Muller, R. T. (2017, July 26). A hands-on approach to psychotherapy? Psychology Today. https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/talking-about-trauma/201707/hands-approach-psychotherapy
National Association of Social Workers. (2017). Code of ethics. Retrieved from https://www.socialworkers.org/About/Ethics/Code-of-Ethics/Code-of-Ethics-English
Niemi, A.K. (2017). Review of randomized controlled trials of massage in preterm infants. Children, 4(4), 21. https://doi.org.10.3390/children4040021
Norwood, D. (2017, June 29). Touch in therapy: helpful or harmful? One therapist’s perspective. Good Therapy. https://www.goodtherapy.org/blog/touch-in-therapy-helpful-or-harmful-one-therapists-perspective-0629175
Novak, E. T. (2017). A model of informed physical contact in psychotherapy. Transactional Analysis Journal, 48(1), 18-32. https://doi.org/10.1080/03621537.2018.1397962
O’Brien, M., & Lynch, H. (2011). Exploring the role of touch in the first year of life: Mothers’ perspectives of tactile interactions with their infants. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 74(3), 129-136. https://doi.org/10.4276/030802211X12996065859247
O’Connor, T. S.-J. (2017). Exploring touch. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 1-9). University of Ottawa Press. https://ruor.uottawa.ca/bitstream/10393/37596/1/9780776627564_WEB.pdf
O’Keefe, D. (2016). Therapeutic relating embodied: An exploration of psychotherapists’ experiences of touch in therapy. [Master’s thesis, Dublin Business School]. https://esource.dbs.ie/bitstream/handle/10788/3074/ma_okeeffe_d_2016.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y
Older, J. (1982). Touching is healing. Stein & Day.
Pederson, P. B., Draguns, J. G., Lonner, W. J., & Trimble, J. E. (Eds.). (1996). Counseling across cultures (4th ed.). Sage Publications, Inc.
Perls, F. (1973). The Gestalt approach and eye witness to therapy. Science and Behavior Books.
Pert, C. (1997). Molecules of emotion. Scribner.
Peterson, M. B. (2001). Recognizing concerns about how some licensing boards are treating psychologists. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 32(4), 339-340. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.32.4.339
Petrella, C., & Rovers, M. (2017). Inter-partner touch in couple counselling: Theory and emerging practice. In M. Rovers, J. Malette, & M. Guirguis-Younger (Eds.), Touch in the helping professions: Research, practice, and ethics (pp. 125-143). University of Ottawa Press. https://ruor.uottawa.ca/bitstream/10393/37596/1/9780776627564_WEB.pdf
Phelan, J. E. (2009). Exploring the use of touch in the psychotherapeutic setting: A phenomenological review. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 46(1), 97-111. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014751
Pinson, B. (2002). Touch in therapy: An effort to make the unknown known. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 32(2-3), 179-196.
Pope, K. S. (1982). Implications of fantasy and imagination for mental health: Theory, research, and interventions. Report commissioned by the National Institute of Mental Health, Order No. 82M024784505D). Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.
Pope, K. S. (1990a). Therapist-patient sex as sex abuse: Six scientific, professional, and practical dilemmas in addressing victimization and rehabilitation. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21(4), 227-239. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.21.4.227
Pope, K. S. (1990b). Therapist-patient sexual contact: Clinical, legal, and ethical implications. In E. A. Margenau (Ed.), The encyclopedic handbook of private practice (pp. 687-696). Gardner Press, Inc.
Pope, K. S., & Bouhoutsos, J. C. (1986). Sexual intimacy between therapists and patients. Praeger.
Pope, K. S., Schover, L. S., & Levenson, H. (1980). Sexual behavior between clinical supervisors and trainees: Implications for standards of professional practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 11, 157-162.
Pope, K. S., Sonne, J. L., & Holroyd, J. (1993). Sexual feelings in psychotherapy: Explorations for therapists and therapists-in-training. American Psychological Association.
Pope, K. S., & Tabachnick, B. G. (1994). Therapists as patients: A national survey of psychologists’ experiences, problems, and beliefs. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25(3), 247-258. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.25.3.247
Pope, K. S., Tabachnick, B. G., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (1987). Ethics of practice: The beliefs and behaviors of psychologists as therapists. American Psychologist, 42(11), 993–1006. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.42.11.993
Pope, K. S., & Vasquez, M. J. T. (1998). Ethics in therapy and counseling: A practical guide (2nd ed.). Jossey-Bass.
Porreca, A., Parolin, M., Bozza, G., Freato, S. & Simonelli, A. (2017). Infant massage and quality of early mother-infant interactions: Are there associations with maternal psychological wellbeing, marital quality, and social support? Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org.10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02049
Prescott, J. W. (1975). Body pleasure and the origins of violence. The Atomic Scientist, 31(9), 10-20. https://doi.org/10.1080/00963402.1975.11458292
Prescott, T. J., Diamond, M. E., & Wing, A. M. (2011). Active touch sensing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1581), 2989-2995. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0167
Redleaf, A. (1998). Behind closed doors: Gender, sexuality, and touch in the doctor/patient relationship. Auburn House.
Reich, W. (1972). Character analysis. Simon & Schuster.
Rogers, C. (1970). Carl Rogers on encounter groups. Harper & Row.
Rothschild, B. (2000). The body remembers: The psychophysiology of trauma and trauma treatment. Norton.
Rubenfeld, I. (2000). The listening hand: Self-healing through the Rubenfeld Synergy Method of talk and touch. Bantam.
Rutter, P. (1989). Sex in the forbidden zone: When men in power betray women’s trust. New York: Psychiatric Annals, 21, 614-619.
Saint Arnault, D., & O’Halloran, S. (2015). Biodynamic psychotherapy for trauma recovery: A pilot study. International Body Psychotherapy Journal: The Art and Science of Somatic Praxis, 14(1), 20-34. Retrieved from http://usabp.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/10/IBPJ-Volume14-No1Spring2015.pdf
Salvio, M., Beutler, L., Wood, J., & Engle, D. (1992). The strength of the therapeutic alliance in three treatments for depression. Psychotherapy Research, 2(1), 31-36. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503309212331333578
Samovar, L., Porter, R., & Jain, N. (1981). Understanding intercultural communication. Wadsworth Publishing Co.
Satir, V. (1972). Peoplemaking. Science & Behavior Books.
Scheflen, A. E. (1972). Body language and the social order: Communication as behavioral control. Prentice-Hall.
Schneider, D. M., & Homans, G. C. (1955). Kinship terminology and the American kinship system. American Anthropologist, 57(6), 1194-1208. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1955.57.6.02a00100
Schultz, L. G. (1975). A survey of social workers’ attitudes and use of body and sex psychotherapies. Clinical Social Work Journal, 3(2), 90-99.
Sehlstedt, I., Ignell, H., Backlund Wasling, H., Ackerley, R., Olausson, H., & Croy, I. (2016). Gentle touch perception across the lifespan. Psychology and Aging, 31(2), 176-184. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000074
Shoghi, M., Sohrabi, S., & Rasouli, M. (2018). The effects of massage by mothers on mother-infant attachment. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine, 24(3), 34-39.
Siegel, D. J. (2010). Mindsight: The new science of personal transformation. Bantam Books.
Simon, R. I. (1991). Psychological injury caused by boundary violation precursors to therapist-patient sex. Psychiatric Annals, 21(10), 614-619. https://doi.org/10.3928/0048-5713-19911001-09
Simon, R. I. (1992). Treatment boundary violations: Clinical, ethical, and legal considerations. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 20, 269-288.
Simon, R. I. (1994). Transference in therapist-patient sex: The illusion of patient improvement and consent, part 1. Psychiatric Annals, 24, 509-515.
Simon, R. I. (1995). The natural history of therapist sexual misconduct: Identification and prevention. Psychiatric Annals, 25(2), 90-94. https://doi.org/10.3928/0048-5713-19950201-07
Slater, V. (1995). Toward an understanding of energetic healing. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 13(3), 209-238.
Smith, E., Clance, P .R., & Imes, S. (Eds.). (1998). Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research and practice. Guilford Press.
Sonne, J. L. (1989). An example of group therapy for victims of therapist-client sexual intimacy. In G. Gabbard (Ed.), Sexual exploitation in professional relationships (pp. 101-114). American Psychiatric Press.
Sonne, J. L. (1994). Multiple relationships: Does the new ethics code answer the right questions? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25(4), 336-343. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.25.4.336
St. Germaine, J. (1996). Dual relationships and certified alcohol and drug counselors: A national study of ethical beliefs and behaviors. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 14(2), 29-45. https://doi.org/10.1300/J020V14N02_03
Steele, K., & Colrain, J. (1990). Abreactive work with sexual abuse survivors: Concepts and techniques. In M. Hunter (Ed.), The sexually abused male: Volume II (pp. 1-55). Lexington Books.
Strasburger, L. H., Jorgenson, L., & Sutherland, P. (1992). The prevention of psychotherapist sexual misconduct: Avoiding the slippery slope. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46(4), 544-555.
Sue, D. W., Ivey, A. E., & Pedersen, P. B. (Eds.). (1996). A theory of multicultural counseling and therapy. Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.
Thayer, S. (1982). Social touching. In W. Schiff & E. Foulke (Eds.), Tactual perception: A sourcebook (pp. 263-304). Cambridge University Press.
Tirnauer, L., Smith, E., & Foster, P. (1996). The American Academy of Psychotherapists Research Committee Survey of members. Voices, 32(2), 87-94.
Tomm, K. (1993). The ethics of dual relationships. The California Therapist, January/February, 7-9.
United States Association for Body Psychotherapy. (2003). Description of Body Psychotherapy. Retrieved from http://www.usabp.org/displaycommon.cfm?an=1&subarticlenbr=30
Van der Kolk, B. A., McFarlane, A. C., & Weisaeth, L. (Eds.). (1996). Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society. The Guilford Press.
von Mohr, M., Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2017). The soothing function of touch: Affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific Reports, 7(13516), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13355-7
Walker, L. E. A. (1994). Abused women and survivor therapy: A practical guide for the psychotherapist. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10153-000
Warnecke, T. (2011). Stirring the depths: transference, countertransference and touch. Body, Movement and Dance in Psychotherapy, 6(3), 233-243. https://doi.org/10.1080/17432979.2011.592390
Watson, J. B. (1924). The psychological care of the infant. University of Chicago Press.
WebMD. (1992). Way too personal: The temptation and consequences of patient-therapist sex. By Colleen Bates Dunn, WebMD feature. Retrieved from http://aolsvc.health.webmd.aol.com/content/Article/14/1687_50119.htm?printing=true
Westland, G. (2015). Verbal and non-verbal communication in psychotherapy. W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
Whitcher, S. J., & Fisher, J. D. (1979). Multidimensional reaction to therapeutic touch in ahospital setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 87-96. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.87
Whitfield, C. L. (1993). Boundaries and relationships: Knowing, protecting and enjoying the self. Health Communications, Inc.
Williams, M. H. (1997). Boundary violations: Do some contended standards of care fail to encompass commonplace procedures of humanistic, behavioral, and eclectic psychotherapies? Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 34(3), 238-249. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087717
Willison, B. G., & Masson, R. L. (1986). The role of touch in psychotherapy: An adjunct to communication. Journal of Counseling & Development, 64(8), 497-500. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1986.tb01180.x
Wilson, J. M. (1982). The value of touch in psychotherapy. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52(1), 65-72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb02665.x
Winnicott, D. W. (1975; 1958). Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis: Collected papers. Karnac London.
Wolberg, L. R. (1967). The technique of psychotherapy (2nd ed.). Grune & Stratton.
Wolfgang, M. E. (1969). Violence and human behavior, In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the American Psychological Association, Washington. D.C., August.
Woody, R. H. (1988). Fifty ways to avoid malpractice: A guidebook for mental health professionals. Professional Resource Exchange, Inc.
Young, C. (2005). About the ethics of professional touch. Retrieved from http://www.eabp.org/pdf/TheEthicsofTouch.pdf.
Young, C., & Westland, G. (2014). Shadows in the history of body psychotherapy: Part I. International Body Psychotherapy Journal: The Art and Science of Somatic Praxis, 13(1), 13-30. Retrieved from http://usabp.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/10/IBPJournal-Vol-13-1-Spring-2014.pdf
Zhang, X., & Wang, J. (2019). Massage intervention for preterm infants by their mothers: A randomized controlled trial. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 24(2), 1-7. https://doi.org.10.1111/jspn. 12238
Zur, O. (1994). Rethinking “Don’t blame the victim”: Psychology of victimhood. Journal of Couples Therapy, 3(4), 15-36. https://doi.org/10.1300/J036v04n03_03
Zur, O. (2000). In celebration of dual relationships: How prohibition of non-sexual dual relationships increases the chance of exploitation and harm. Independent Practitioner, 2(3), 97-100.
Zur, O. (2001). Out-of-office experience: When crossing office boundaries and engaging in dual relationships are clinically beneficial and ethically sound. Independent Practitioner, 21(1), 96-100.
Zur, O. (2005). Dumbing down of psychology: Manufactured consent about the depravity of dual relationships in therapy. In R. H. Wright & N. A. Cummings, (Eds.), Destructive trends in mental health: The well-intentioned road to harm (pp. 253-282). Brunner-Routledge.
Zur, O. (2007a). Touch in therapy. In O. Zur, (Ed.), Boundaries in psychotherapy: Ethical and clinical explorations (pp. 167-185). APA Books. https://doi.org/10.1037/11563-000
Zur, O. (2007b). Touch in therapy and the standard of care in psychotherapy and counseling: Bringing clarity to illusive relationships. U.S. Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal, 6(2), 61-93.
Zur, O. (2009). Re-thinking the “power differential” in psychotherapy: Exploring the myth of therapists’ omnipotence and patients’ fragility. Voice: The Art and Science of Psychotherapy, 45(1).
Zur, O. (2015). Ethical and legal aspects of touch in psychotherapy. Retrieved from https://www.zurinstitute.com/ethicsoftouch.html.
Top of Page